Neglectful Parenting and Personality Traits as Predictors of Malevolent Creativity among Ghanaian Tertiary Education Students

Abstract

Creative potentials are diverse in humans and can be manifested when influenced by other unobservable and observable factors. In light of this, we examined the predictive roles of neglectful parenting and personality traits on malevolent creativity. The cross-sectional design was used to survey 623 participants. Data for the study were collected with adapted neglectful parenting, personality traits, and malevolent creativity scales. The data were analysed inferentially with CB-SEM. The study revealed positive predictions of physical and emotional neglect on hurting people, lying, and playing tricks as aspects of malevolent creativity. Furthermore, conscientious personality trait predicted higher on hurting people, lying, and playing tricks as aspects of malevolent creativity. We concluded that neglectful parenting and improper parenting can bring about unacceptable behaviours among people as many of the victims might become a social canker. Therefore, families and significant others in communities should be taken through strategies in preventing irregular parenting and moderated personality dispositions before they develop into unacceptable behaviours that might ruin the gains of society.

Share and Cite:

Bedu-Addo, P. , Mahama, I. , Amoako, B. , Amos, P. and Antwi, T. (2023) Neglectful Parenting and Personality Traits as Predictors of Malevolent Creativity among Ghanaian Tertiary Education Students. Creative Education, 14, 232-244. doi: 10.4236/ce.2023.142016.

1. Introduction

Creativity is a recognized latent trait that is accepted as good for every human being. Creativity is defined as an individual’s ability to create unique and useful things (Sternberg & Lubart, 1996) . Some scholars explained creativity using concepts such as product, personal, process, and place (Rhodes, 1961) while others termed creativity to be personal motivation, personal attributes, and personal feelings (Cropley et al., 2014; Cropley & Cropley, 2010) . Despite this, some aspects of creativity human beings are noted to be murkier because they involve doubtful motives. This is to say that, the wish for succeeding at all cost in any act using any doubtful strategy is nothing but an issue of malevolent creativity. Malevolent creativity is an act of been smart, tricky, negative, and egocentric at the expense of others in the same situation or different situations. Malevolent creativity is the negative and anti-social side of creativity (Hao et al., 2016) . In the view of Cropley et al. (2008: p. 106) , malevolent creativity was defined as “a deliberate plan to damage people for personal gains or otherwise”. Among the several malevolent behaviours exhibited by people, the most recognizable ones are dishonesty, lying, bullying, and theft with the goal to harm others (Harris & Reiter-Palmon, 2015; Wang, 2018) . Although malevolent creativity is known to exist in people, its precursors, however are not well established because of the diverse opinions emanating from creativity as a concept. In some literature, malevolent creativity often occurs in people with aggressive tendencies (Hao et al., 2016) , situations where unjust and intimidating social structures are available (Cheng, Baas, & De Dreu, 2018) , and when people anticipate failure or discomfort (Woody & Szechtman, 2011) . Taken together, malevolent behaviours occur as a result of predisposition to aggressiveness and facing impending harmful consequences in the environment. Based on these, it is important to note that malevolent creativity results from social threats (Baas, Roskes, Koch, Cheng, & De Dreu, 2019) . In as much as social factors could determine malevolent creativity, they cannot be given a complete prominence because they seem general, extensive, and inconclusive. To bring the case closer to an acceptable conclusion, it is important to consider home factors such as neglectful parenting and human stable patterns of behaviour such as personality traits.

Neglectful parenting is the unconscious or conscious failure of immediate and extended caregivers to meet the needs of their children or lack of care on the part of caregivers to their offspring (Golden, Samuels, & Southall, 2003) . Neglectful parenting can be abuse such as physical, sexual, and emotional. Neglectful parenting may compel people to exhibit malevolent creative behaviours such as lying, hurting people, and playing tricks with others (Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002; Infurna et al., 2016; Jia et al., 2020; Jennissen et al., 2016) . In a study, Csathó and Birkás (2018) found that harmful childhood practices emanating from irregular parental care could cause cynical and hateful interactive style like malevolent creativity. In the study, Jia et al. (2020) assessed 991 undergraduate students in China concerning neglect from childhood, stable traits like personality, and malevolent creativity. Jia et al. found that childhood neglect positively influenced students’ malevolent creative behaviours, such as being vindictive, conjuring situations, and behaving unpleasantly.

Child abuse and neglect has become a public health, social and child rights issue for all nations (WHO, 2022; Zevulun et al., 2019) . Logan-Greene and Jones (2015) conducted a longitudinal study and found that enduring childhood neglect influenced aggression, which in turn reduces people’s social functioning. For example, parental physical neglect can affect students’ cognitive functioning, hence the show of malevolent creativity in terms of hurting people (Kavanaugh et al., 2017) . Furthermore, parental neglect, whether physical or emotional, can lead students developing post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In this regard, the affected individuals may engage in fearful, troubled, anger, guilt, or shameful behaviours (Sege et al., 2017) , which may in turn lead them to show malevolent creative actions.

Aside from neglectful parenting behaviours influencing malevolent creativity, extant literature shows that personality traits also predict malevolent creative behaviours (Zhe & Chang, 2016) . Specifically, Lee and Dow (2011) study findings showed that low levels of conscientiousness personality traits influenced malevolent creativity. Similarly, Low and Dow found openness personality trait to be a positive predictor of malevolent creativity among people. In situations where malevolent creativity was converted to divergent thinking, it was found that malevolent creative ideas are invoked and these correlated positively with personality traits (Batey et al., 2022; Gutworth & Hunter, 2017; Kapoor & Khan, 2020) . Taking together, extant literature indicates strong relationship among neglectful parenting, personality traits and malevolent creativity. In this regard, the current study drives on the assumption that neglectful parenting and personality traits could predict malevolent creations in the academic lives of tertiary education students in Ghana.

1.1. Theoretical Review

The study dwells on the life history theory and interaction model of creativity. Kaplan and Gangestad (2005) coined the life history theory to explain the unexpected social and environmental factors that could change the normal course of growth and actions of people. Scholars have alleged that when such unexpected and harsh environmental factors present themselves in the absence of resources that serve as a buffer, they create an opportunity for people to increase in their physiological adjustment (Belsky et al., 2010; Csathó & Birkás, 2018) . In this instance, those affected might resort to behaviours and actions that might be inimical and contrary to societal norms and standards (Cabeza de Baca et al., 2016; Chang & Lu, 2018) . According to Jia et al. (2020) , such behaviours depict aspects of personality traits (Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and narcissism) and malevolent behaviours. Deductively, neglectful parenting could come from a poor environment and at the same time, stable patterns of behaviour could manifest due to the poor environment in which an individual finds themselves.

Woodman and Schoenfeldt (1990) propounded a model called interactionist creativity. This model provides an opportunity for people to consider diversity in creativity among individuals. This diversity in creativity may occur through conditions such as socialisation, family status, mental dispositions, and contextual markers. The interactionist model of creativity also notes that neglectful parenting, malevolent creativity and dark triad personality could evolve from such conditions (Infurna et al., 2016) because many of those experiencing such conditions in a negative dimension might not be able to control their emotions (Jennissen et al., 2016) .

1.2. The Current Study

Contemporary educational, social, and economic demands have increased interest in teachers and learners’ creative abilities (de Arriba et al., 2019; Joynes et al., 2019; Mahama, 2022; Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development [OECD], 2018 ). However, it is important to note that creative abilities may not always help solve problems but also cause problems to others in society through malevolent creativity (Cropley et al., 2008) . In as much as malevolent creativity is inevitable in the human race, empirical studies allude to the fact that neglectful parenting and personality traits are in tandem with malevolent-based psychological processes. Several empirical research such as Jankowska et al. (2018) and Moltafet et al. (2018) have favoured family-related factors in promoting or impeding the process of creativity among people. However, it appears little to no study has investigated the influence of specific parental factors (e.g. physical and emotional neglect) on the malevolent creativity of students. Again, aspects of personality traits such as antithetical, aggressiveness, antagonism, aggression, and sympathy are favoured as predictors of malevolent creativity (Bettencourt et al., 2006; Eisenberg, 2000; Lee & Dow, 2011) , however, less is known about how the Big-Five personality traits predict malevolent creativity among students. Furthermore, the choice of a sample group in studies concerning neglectful parenting, malevolent creativity, and personality traits across the world seems to have been those of pre-tertiary education and parents than those at the tertiary level (Odame-Mensah & Gyimah, 2018; Steele et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2022) . However, students at the tertiary level are those believed to exhibit malevolent behaviours resulting from neglectful parebting because they regard themselves as independent and could act on their desires with less restrictions (Jia, et al., 2020; Li et al., 2022) . As a result, we anticipate that parental physical and emotional neglect and components of the Big-Five personality traits (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) may explain malevolent creative behaviours (hurting, lying, and playing tricks) among tertiary education students in Ghana. In view of the foregoing arguments, the study dwelled on the following hypotheses:

H1: Neglectful parenting will predict malevolent creativity among tertiary education students.

H2: Personality traits will predict malevolent creativity among tertiary education students.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

The study surveyed 623 (male = 355, female = 268) higher education students in a Ghanaian Public University. The participants were undergraduate students pursuing diverse academic programmes. These participants were presumed to have in one way or other expereicned some form of a typical Ghanaian parenting (e.g. external/autoritarian) with its associated social, economic, financial, and economic conditions.The participants were drawn from level 100 to level 400 (level 100 = 138, level 200 = 213, level 300 = 173 and level 400 = 99). The participants were contacted through the university virtual classroom (VClass). The needed data was gathered between July 15, 2022 and October 25, 2022. The online method provided the participants with ample time and convenience for responding to the survey.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Parental Neglect

In measuring neglectful parenting (16-items) among the participants, two sub-scales (emotional neglect and physical neglect) of the child abuse scale (Malik & Shah, 2007) were adapted. The two subscales were chosen because they reflect how Ghanaian parenting is practised. Emotional neglect had 12-items (e.g. “My parents spend time with me”) while physical neglect had 4-items (e.g. “None of my parental care is received when sick”). The scale was scored based on a 4-point Likert-type scale from never = 1 to always = 4. Although the scale has an established and acceptable internal consistency from previous studies, we found an internal consistency of .83 using Cronbach’s Alpha procedure.

2.2.2. Personality Traits

The participants’ personality traits were established with an adapted short version of the Big-Five personality inventory [BFI-10] (Rammstedt & John, 2007) . All five subscales were used. Each subscale had 2-items each with an item scored reversely. The subscales are openness to experience (e.g. “I see myself as someone who has an active imagination”), conscientiousness (“I see myself as someone who tends to be lazy”), extraversion (“I see myself as someone who has few artistic interests”), agreeableness (“I see myself as someone who tends to find fault with others”), and neuroticism (“I see myself as someone who is relaxed, handles stress well”). The responses were scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale from strongly disagree = 1 to strongly agree = 4. The internal consistency of the scale is well documented in several empirical literatures yet we found an appreciable internal consistency of .76 using the Cronbach’s Alpha procedure.

2.3. Malevolent Creativity

Malevolent creative potentials in the participants were measured by an adapted 13-item malevolent creativity scale (Hao et al., 2016) . The malevolent creativity was measured in three domains: hurting people (e.g. 6-items, “How often do you think about ideas to take revenge when being unfairly treated?”), lying (e.g. 4-items, “How often do you fabricate lies to simplify a problem situation?”), and playing tricks (e.g. 3-items, “How often do you have ideas about how to pull pranks on others?”). In each domain, responses were scored based on a 4-point Likert-type scale from never = 1 to usually = 4. Irrespective of the already established internal consistency of the scale, we found an appreciable internal consistency of .74 using the Cronbach’s Alpha procedure.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

We performed regression using the covariance-based structural equation modelling strategy (CB-SEM). In this analysis, the exogenous variables were neglectful parenting and personality traits, while the endogenous variable was malevolent creativity. The choice of this analysis procedure was informed by its robust nature to establish statistical effects with conditions to control for statistical errors.

3. Results

H1: Neglectful parenting will predict malevolent creative behaviours among tertiary education students.

The focus of the hypothesis was on the predictive ability of emotional and physical neglect on hurting, lying, and playing tricks with people. The process of the predictions was established uisng CB-SEM strategy. Figure 1 and Table 1 present the results.

Table 1 shows the results on aspects of neglectful parenting predicting aspects of malevolent creativity among higher education students. The results revealed that all the exogenous variables produced positive predictions. Using playing tricks as the endogenous variable, emotional neglect (b = .612, p < .000) predicted higher than physical neglect (b = .211, p < .000) in playing tricks with people by the participants. Using hurting people as the endogenous variable,

Figure 1. PN predicting MVC.

emotional neglect (b = .592, p < .000) was predicted higher than physical neglect (b = .204, p < .000) in hurting behaviours of the participants. Using lying to people as the endogenous variable, emotional neglect (b = .601, p < .000) predicted higher than physical neglect (b = .208, p < .000) in lying to people by the participants. The findings imply that participants whose parents felt no need to offer them emotional and physical support as they grew up from the environment could lead them to show some forms of tricky behaviours; becoming perpetual liars, and may hurt others as they interact with them.

H2: Personality traits will predict malevolent creative behaviours among higher education students.

The focus of the hypothesis was on the predictive abilities of openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism on hurting, lying, and playing tricks with people. The process of the predictions was established using CB-SEM strategy. Figure 2 and Table 2 present the results.

Table 1. Neglectful parenting and malevolent creativity.

Table 2. Personality traits predicting malevolent creativity.

Figure 2. PT predicting MVC.

Table 2 shows the results on personality traits predicting malevolent creativity among higher education students. The results revealed that all the exogenous variables predicted positively on the endogenous. Using lying to people as the endogenous variable, agreeableness predicted higher (b = .230, p < .000) than neuroticism (b = .227, p < .000), conscientiousness (b = .26, p < .000), openness (b = .211, p < .000), and extraversion (b = .210, p < .000). Using hurting people as the endogenous variable, agreeableness predicted higher (b = .218, p < .000) than neuroticsm (b = .215, p < .000), conscientiousness (b = .213, p < .000), openness (b = .200, p < .000), and extraversion (b = .198, p < .000). Using playing tricks on people as the endogenous variable, agreeableness predicted higher (b = .291, p < .000), neuroticism (b = .287, p < .000), conscientiousness (b = .285, p < .000), openness (b = .267, p < .000), and extraversion (b = .265, p < .000). Among the five dimensions of the personality traits, agreeableness predicted higher on the dimensions of malevolent creativity.

4. Discussion

In as much as creativity is regarded a vital component of students’ life, it has other aspects that might be distructive to students’ progress. Again, human’s ability to creatively outwit others have noted through terrorism, weapons of mass distraction and burglary. However, precursor such behaviours have been subtle in literature. Therefore, our study examined neglectful parenting and personality traits predicting malevolent creativity. Given this, our findings show a positive significant prediction of neglectful parenting and personality traits on malevolent creativity. These support previous objectives that examined neglectful parenting and personality traits as explanatory variables to malevolent creative potentials of human beings (Kapoor & Khan, 2020; Kavanaugh et al., 2017; Logan-Greene & Jones, 2015; Zhe & Chang, 2016) . Our study findings imply that, those denied of their physical and emotional needs might resort to hurting others, lying to others and deceiving or applying tricks in their interactions with people. Improper behaviours emanating from improper parental are discussed in several literature reviews. For example, it is alleged that harmful parental practices could lead children to exhibit contemptuous and intolerable interactive behaviours (Csathó & Birkás, 2018; Jia et al., 2020) . In as much malevolent creative behaviours might be evident when people suffer from neglectful parenting, such individuals may experience other social problems like aggression and reduction in social functioning (Logan-Greene & Jones, 2015; WHO, 2022) .

Again, our study findings show that personality traits could influence malevolent creative behaviours of individuals. For examples, people who give room for other influencing factors in their lives can easily resort malevolent creative behaviours. Even in some instances, individuals who are well organised, have control over themselves and exhibit awareness in their interactions could give in to dark aspects of creativity after winning courting the trust of people. When this happens, it is more or less conscientious behaviours. In fact, conscientious behaviours are among the most prominent aspects of people that drive malevolent creativity. For example, in their study, Lee and Dow (2011) found conscientiousness and openness personality traits as positive predictors of malevolent creativity, especially when conscientious was found to be low in those affected. Furthermore, our study findings corroborate with previous studies that found personality traits to be positive explanatory variables to malevolent creative behaviours among people (Batey et al., 2022; Gutworth & Hunter, 2017; Kapoor & Khan, 2020) .

5. Conclusion and Recommendations

Abuse of children, neglectful parenting and a compromised personality trait may lead to a wide range of adverse consequences for all individuals from infancy to adulthood. Neglectful parenting may have debilitating and prolonged effects on people, as many of the victims might become a social canker in terms of malevolent behaviours, which could bring economic cost on nations, especially when the law is broken through malevolent actions. When people experience neglectful parenting, it could bring about lifetime psychological consequences (attachment problems, poor mental and emotional health, PTSD, and substance use). The effects can also present themselves as educational difficulties, low self-esteem, depression, and trouble forming and maintaining relationships (Young & Widom, 2014) .

To ameliorate the effects of malevolent creative behaviours, stakeholders in society can serve as a buffer to parenting so that the effects of neglectful parenting be reduced. The literature supports the role of the society in mitigating malevolent creativity. For instance, Ram (2012) showed the value of informal social control in preventing and restraining people from exhibiting malevolent creations. In situations where people exhibit these malevolent creations, they could be supported and reintegrated into acceptable ways of behaving regardless of their stable traits and upbringing.

Welfare organisations could also help mitigate issues of parenting, personality, and malevolent creations affecting people. They can do this by taking families and significant others in communities through strategies to prevent irregular parenting and moderated personality dispositions before they develop into unacceptable behaviours that align with creativity’s dark aspects. This could be an issue of psychosocial management, which demands an appreciable understanding, complementary and coordination roles between communities, stakeholders, and service providers (Shenoi et al., 2013; Shipley & Arrigo, 2012) . It is important to note that parenting in Ghana appears to be authoritarian and lacks warmth; hence, malevolent creations are inevitable among people (Huang et al., 2018; Therborn, 2006) . Nonetheless, such malevolent behaviours could be mitigated by capitalizing on the affected’s strengths and helping address their pertinent needs.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

References

[1] Baas, M., Roskes, M., Koch, S., Cheng, Y., & De Dreu, C. K. (2019). Why Social Threat Motivates Malevolent Creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45, 1590-1602.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219838551
[2] Batey, M., Hughes, D. J., Mosley, A., Owens, C. E., & Furnham, A. (2022). Psychopathy and Openness-to-Experience as Predictors of Malevolent and Benevolent Creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 196, Article ID: 111715.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111715
[3] Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., Houts, R. M., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2010). The Development of Reproductive Strategy in Females: Early Maternal Harshness → Earlier menarche → Increased Sexual Risk Taking. Developmental Psychology, 46, 120.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015549
[4] Bettencourt, B., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and Aggressive Behavior under Provoking and Neutral Conditions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.751
[5] Cabeza de Baca, T., Wahl, R. A., Barnett, M. A., Figueredo, A. J., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). Adversity, Adaptive Calibration, and Health: The Case of Disadvantaged Families. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2, 93-115.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0042-z
[6] Chang, L., & Lu, H. J. (2018). Resource and Extrinsic Risk in Defining Fast Life Histories of Rural Chinese Left-Behind Children. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 39, 59-66.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.003
[7] Cheng, Y., Baas, M., & De Dreu, C. K. (2018). Creative Responses to Imminent Threats: The Role of Threat Direction and Perceived Effectiveness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 174-186.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.013
[8] Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent Creativity: A Functional Model of Creativity in Terrorism and Crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 105-115.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410802059424
[9] Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., White, A. E., & Chiera, B. A. (2014). Layperson Perceptions of Malevolent Creativity: The Good, the Bad, and the Ambiguous. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 400-412.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037792
[10] Cropley, D., & Cropley, A. (2010). Functional Creativity: “Products” and the Generation of Effective Novelty. In The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 301-317). Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511763205.019
[11] Csathó, á., & Birkás, B. (2018). Early-Life Stressors, Personality Development, and Fast Life Strategies: An Evolutionary Perspective on Malevolent Personality Features. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 305.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00305
[12] de Arriba, R., Girardi, G., & Vidagan, M. (2019). Contemporary Art in Higher Education: Creative Pedagogies in Political Economy. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 33, Article ID: 100577.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2019.100577
[13] Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, Regulation, and Moral Development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665-697.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665
[14] Golden, M. H., Samuels, M. P., & Southall, D. P. (2003). How to Distinguish between Neglect and Deprivational Abuse. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 88, 105-107.
https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.88.2.105
[15] Gutworth, M. B., & Hunter, S. T. (2017). Ethical Saliency: Deterring Deviance in Creative Individuals. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 428.
https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000093
[16] Hao, N., Tang, M., Yang, J., Wang, Q., & Runco, M. A. (2016). A New Tool to Measure Malevolent Creativity: The Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 682.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00682
[17] Harris, D. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2015). Fast and Furious: The Influence of Implicit Aggression, Premeditation, and Provoking Situations on Malevolent Creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 54-64.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038499
[18] Hildyard, K. L., & Wolfe, D. A. (2002). Child Neglect: Developmental Issues and Outcomes. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 679-695.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00341-1
[19] Huang, K. Y., Bornheimer, L. A., Dankyi, E., & de-Graft Aikins, A. (2018). Parental Wellbeing, Parenting and Child Development in Ghanaian Families with Young Children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 49, 833-841.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-018-0799-3
[20] Infurna, M. R., Reichl, C., Parzer, P., Schimmenti, A., Bifulco, A., & Kaess, M. (2016). Associations between Depression and Specific Childhood Experiences of Abuse and Neglect: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 47-55.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.006
[21] Jankowska, D. M., Czerwonka, M., Lebuda, I., & Karwowski, M. (2018). Exploring the Creative Process: Integrating Psychometric and Eye-Tracking Approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1931.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01931
[22] Jennissen, S., Holl, J., Mai, H., Wolff, S., & Barnow, S. (2016). Emotion Dysregulation Mediates the Relationship between Child Maltreatment and Psychopathology: A Structural Equation Model. Child Abuse & Neglect, 62, 51-62.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.10.015
[23] Jia, X., Wang, Q., & Lin, L. (2020). The Relationship between Childhood Neglect and Malevolent Creativity: The Mediating Effect of the Dark Triad Personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article ID: 613695.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.613695
[24] Joynes, C., Rossignoli, S., & Amonoo-Kuofi, E. F. (2019). 21st Century Skills: Evidence of Issues in Definition, Demand and Delivery for Development Contexts (pp. 1-75). Emerging Issues Report.
[25] Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life History Theory and Evolutionary Psychology: The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. John Wiley and Sons.
[26] Kapoor, H., & Khan, A. (2020). Creators and Presses: The Person-Situation Interaction in Negative Creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54, 75-89.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.346
[27] Kavanaugh, B. C., Dupont-Frechette, J. A., Jerskey, B. A., & Holler, K. A. (2017). Neurocognitive Deficits in Children and Adolescents Following Maltreatment: Neurodevelopmental Consequences and Neuropsychological Implications of Traumatic Stress. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6, 64-78.
https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2015.1079712
[28] Lee, S. A., & Dow, G. T. (2011). Malevolent Creativity: Does Personality Influence Malicious Divergent Thinking? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 73-82.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.571179
[29] Li, W., Zhang, L., Qin, Z., Chen, J., & Liu, C. (2022). Childhood Trauma and Malevolent Creativity in Chinese College Students: Moderated Mediation by Psychological Resilience and Aggression. Journal of Intelligence, 10, 1-16.
https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040097
[30] Logan-Greene, P., & Jones, A. S. (2015). Chronic Neglect and Aggression/Delinquency: A Longitudinal Examination. Child Abuse & Neglect, 45, 9-20.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.04.003
[31] Mahama, I. (2022). Creative Teaching as a Component of the New Standard-Based Curriculum in Ghana: Curriculum Rushed or Curriculum Planned? Mediterranean Journal of Social & Behavioural Research, 6, 27-33.
https://doi.org/10.30935/mjosbr/11569
[32] Malik, F. D., & Shah, A. A. (2007). Development of Child Abuse Scale: Reliability and Validity Analyses. Psychology and Developing Societies, 19, 161-178.
https://doi.org/10.1177/097133360701900202
[33] Moltafet, G., Sadati Firoozabadi, S. S., & Pour-Raisi, A. (2018). Parenting Style, Basic Psychological Needs, and Emotional Creativity: A Path Analysis. Creativity Research Journal, 30, 187-194.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2018.1446748
[34] Odame-Mensah, S., & Gyimah, E. K. (2018). The Role of Permissive and Neglectful Parenting Style in Determining the Academic Performance of Adolescents in the Senior High Schools in the Birim Municipality. Journal of Education and Practice, 9, 73-82.
[35] Organisation for Economic Co-Operation and Development (OECD) (2018). The Future of Education and Skills: Education 2030. OECD Education Working Papers.
[36] Ram, C. D. (2012). Meeting the Challenge of Crime in the Global Village: An Assessment of the Role and Future of the United Nations Commission on the Crime Prevention and Criminal Justice.
[37] Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring Personality in One Minute or Less: A 10 Item Short Version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203-212.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001
[38] Rhodes, M. (1961). An Analysis of Creativity. The Phi Delta Kappan, 42, 305-310.
[39] Sege, R. D., Amaya-Jackson, L., Flaherty, E. G., Idzerda, S. M., Legano, L. A., Leventhal, J. M., & American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Child Abuse and Neglect (2017). Clinical Considerations Related to the Behavioral Manifestations of Child Maltreatment. Pediatrics, 139, e20170100.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-0100
[40] Shenoi, R., Greeley, C., & Giardino, A. P. (2013). Child Maltreatment Prevention-Finding Common Ground with Unintentional Injury Prevention. Journal of Applied Research on Children, 4, 1-31.
[41] Shipley, S. L., & Arrigo, B. A. (2012). Introduction to Forensic Psychology: Court, Law Enforcement, and Correctional Practices. Academic Press.
[42] Steele, K. R., Townsend, M. L., & Grenyer, B. F. (2020). Parenting Stress and Competence in Borderline Personality Disorder Is Associated with Mental Health, Trauma History, Attachment and Reflective Capacity. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 7, 1-14.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-020-00124-8
[43] Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in Creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677-688.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.7.677
[44] Therborn, G. (2006). African Families in a Global Context (Research Report No. 131). Nordiska Afrikainstitutet.
[45] W.H.O. World Health Organisation (2022). Child Maltreatment.
https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/child-maltreatment
[46] Wang, D., Wang, D., & Chen, W. (2022). The Relationship between Adolescents’ Resilience and Their Malevolent Creative Behaviours. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54, 154.
https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00154
[47] Wang, M. Z. (2018). Malevolent Creativity: A Cross-Cultural Study. Honors Thesis Collection 569.
https://repository.wellesley.edu/thesiscollection/569
[48] Woodman, R. W., & Schoenfeldt, L. F. (1990). An Interactionist Model of Creative Behavior. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 24, 279-290.
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.1990.tb00549.x
[49] Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2011). Adaptation to Potential Threat: The Evolution, Neurobiology, and Psychopathology of the Security Motivation System. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1019-1033.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.003
[50] Young, J. C., & Widom, C. S. (2014). Long-Term Effects of Child Abuse and Neglect on Emotion Processing in Adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 38, 1369-1381.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.03.008
[51] Zevulun, D., Post, W. J., Zijlstra, A. E., Kalverboer, M. E., & Knorth, E. J. (2019). The Best Interests of the Child from Different Cultural Perspectives: Factors Influencing Judgements of the Quality of Child-Rearing Environment and Construct Validity of the Best Interests of the Child-Questionnaire (BIC-Q) in Kosovo and Albania. Child Indicators Research, 12, 331-351.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-018-9543-6
[52] Zhe, G., & Chang, L. (2016). Malevolent Creativity: Concept, Measurement, Influence Factors and Future Research. Journal of Psychological Science, 39, 63-75.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.