Myths and Misconceptions in Developmental and Neuro-Psychology ()
1. Introduction
There is a long-standing academic interest in the belief in psychological myths and misconceptions about the subject (Brown, 1983; Furnham, 1992, 1993; Kowalski & Taylor, 2009; Nixon, 1925; Lamal, 1979; McKeachie, 1960; Taylor & Kowalski, 2004; Tupper & Williams, 1986; Vaughan, 1977) . Studies have assessed student’s psychological knowledge with a view to evaluating the success of introductory courses, namely the increase in knowledge and reduction of myth acceptance (Arntzen, Lokke, Lokke, & Eilertsen, 2010; Gardner & Dalsing, 1986; Griggs & Ransdell, 1987; Lamal, 1979; McKeachie, 1960; Standing & Huber, 2003; Vaughan, 1977) . Generally, results have been consistent, showing high levels of misconceptions (40% - 70%) prior to education, which are reduced, but only slightly, following education methods (LaCaille, 2015) . This has surprised and disappointed educationists.
This research area has been galvanized by a very popular book by Lilienfeld, Lynn, Ruscio, and Beyerstein (2010) , 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology. The 50 myths cover diverse areas of psychology, ranging from myths about the brain and perception to myths concerning psychology and the law. They also presented 250 other “mythlets” in need of exploration, which they suggest serving as a useful list of urban legends, misconceptions and non-proven assertions to investigate. Numerous studies have used the 50 myths and 250 mythlets to test various hypotheses in this area (Furnham & Hughes, 2014; Swami et al., 2015) . Other recent books, in a series on psychological myths, have been published looking at specific myths (De Bruyckere, Kirschner, & Hulshof, 2015) .
Further, the Lilienfeld et al. (2010) book has spurred on researchers to develop new scales. Gardner and Brown (2013) developed a new 55-item measure of psychological misconceptions which had good internal consistency. They found overall scores were significantly and negatively correlated with reported reading of news magazines. Bensley, Lilienfeld and Powell (2014) developed a 40-item measure based on the same source and showed that belief in psychological misconceptions was predicted by measures of paranormal belief, faith in intuition, the ability to distinguish scientific fields and practices from pseudoscientific ones, and academic scores.
There has however been little exploration of the personal correlates of those holding myths and misconceptions in psychology. That is, not many studies in this area have looked at demographic (age, education, sex) or psychographic correlates of misconceptions so it is not clear if some groups are more prone to holding myths than others.
The Lilienfeld et al., (2010) book also inspired other various more specialized books with a very similar title and format. This study is based on two of these books. The first was entitled Great Myths of the Brain which listed 41 myths under various headings like defunct myths, immortal myths, myths about the physical structure of the brain (Jarrett, 2014) . The second was Great Myths of Child Development and listed 50 myths under various headings such as growth, body, mind; emotions and behavior and social environment. The study followed the methodology of Furnham and Hughes (2014) who asked participants to rate each myth as either Definitely True, Probably True, Probably False, Definitely False or Don’t Know. As studies have done with the Lilienfeld et al., (2010) book, we used the same wording as that in the book.
We hypothesized, based on previous results, that for over 50% of the myths in both categories participants would rate them as Definitely or Probably True. That is, participants would endorse popular myths. We also hypothesized that those with some education in psychology would have a higher correct score, namely recognizing the myth as such. Thus there would be a positive correlation between psychological education and the rejection of other myths. We explored other various correlates of misconceptions such as sex, age and ideology but entertained no specific hypotheses about the relationship.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
A total of 220 participants completed the two questionnaires. 51.8% were men and 48.2% were women. They ranged in age from 19 to 66 yrs, with the mean age was 35.74 years (SD = 10.89 years). In all 76.4% identified themselves as White, 9.1% Black, 6.8% Asian and 5.9% Hispanic. In all 27.3% had secondary school education and 59.1% were graduates. 46.4% of the respondents had children. Thirty-four percent (267) had undertaken some previous study of psychology or psychiatry at school, college or university. They are rated themselves on three scales: Compared to other people, how much common sense do you believe you have (Very Little 1 to 8 = Very Much) and they scored 6.11 (SD = 1.32); How religious are you? (Not all at 1 to Very 8) and they scored 3.98 (SD = 2.06); and How would you describe your political beliefs? (Very Left Wing 1 to Very Right Wing 8); they scored 3.99 (SD = 2.08).
2.2. Measures
The myths and misconceptions were derived from two books, in series, of myths in psychology: Developmental Psychology (Hupp & Jewell, 2015) and Brain Psychology (Jarrett, 2014) . Response options were broken down into “Probably” and “Definitely” True or False allowing for greater information to be gleaned regarding the kinds of True and False responses. In addition, the “Don’t Know” option improves upon some previous tests as participants could indicate a lack of knowledge, rather than guessing or leaving items unanswered (Arntzen et al., 2010) . This followed the same successful format of Furnham and Hughes (2014) .
2.3. Procedure
Departmental ethical approval was first obtained. Data was gathered using M-Turk which is a crowdsourcing internet marketplace allowing individuals to complete online tasks in exchange for monetary compensation. MTurk samples have been shown to be more demographically-diverse compared to standard internet samples and the website is considered to be a source of high-quality data for social science research (e.g., Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011 ) data collection. The questionnaire took an average of 40 minutes to complete. Data were inspected and cleaned before analysis resulting in the loss of 30 people from the original sample of 250.
3. Results
3.1. Prevalence of Misconceptions
All of the items presented were myths, thus for all items, the “correct” answer was False (Probably or Definitely). Participants’ False responses were summed in order to create a myth recognition score for each of the two questionnaires.
Item level analyses
Brain Myths: Table 1 shows the results for the 56 Brain myths. In all 15 were identified by more than 50% of the participants as Definitely False; that is that they were actually recognized as myths. The top five were 5: Drilling a hole in the head releases evil spirits (91%); 2: The brain pumps animal spirits around the body (81%); 40: Strokes only affect old people (81%); 39: A concussion is not a form of brain injury (71%); and 41: The effects of strokes are limited to physical ability (72%).
On the other hand there were four items where over 40% of the respondents said that the following items were Definitely True: 21: The brain is very well designed (57%); 35: After head injury, people can forget who they are and not recognize others, but be normal in every other way (51%); 5: The Brain receives information from the five separate senses (43%) and 2: Brain cells join together forming a huge net of nerves (41%).
On various items a quarter or more of the respondents said that they Don’t Know; 24: Glial cells are little more than brain “glue” (54%); 25: Mirror neurons make us human (52%); 38: Epilepsy is caused by abnormalities in blood flow in the brain (34%) and 13: The female brain is more balanced than the male brain (25.9%).
Developmental Myths: Table 2 shows the results for the 49 Developmental myths. In all 3 were identified by more than 50% of the participants as Definitely False. They were 8: The Chinese lunar year calendar accurately predicts the sex of a baby (69%); 3: Parents can predict the sex of a fetus by examining the shape of a mother’s body (64%); and 2: Couples dealing with infertility are more likely to get pregnant if they adopt (50%). On the other hand there were two items where over 30% of the respondents said were Definitely True: 4: All boys have one Y chromosome and girls do not (41%); and 7: A woman who is already pregnant can’t get pregnant again (37%).
On eleven items a quarter or more of the respondents said that they Don’t Know. The top five were: 9: Female fetuses have faster heart rates than male fetuses (45%); 45: Parent-child relationship disruptions can be repaired with holding therapy (39%) and 23: Facilitated communication is effective for children with autism (35%).
3.2. Correlates
Two scores were then computed for each individual: the total scores in each questionnaire in which they had marked Definitely False, meaning essentially a correct or well-informed score. For the first questionnaire the score was 18.89 (SD = 8.12) and the second 17.79 (SD = 8.12). The correlation between the two was r = .79 indicating considerable similarity in responses to the two questionnaires.
These scores were then correlated with various of the demographic and belief
Table 1. Results from the first questionnaire.
a. DT = Definitely True, PT = Probably True, PF = Probably False, DF = Definitely False, DK = Don’t Know.
Table 2. Results from the second questionnaire.
a. DT = Definitely True, PT = Probably True, PF = Probably False, DF = Definitely False, DK = Don’t Know.
variables. There was no relationship between either total score and age, gender, education, whether they had children and whether or not they had taken a course in psychology. Indeed, scores of those who had and had not some education in psychology were almost identical. Three variables were significant for both total scores: they indicated the more religious people were the higher their score: test 1, r = 0.24, test 2, r =0.19; more politically conservative had a higher score (test 1, r = 0.23; test 2, r = 0.18) and the more they believed they had Common Sense the higher their score (test 1, r = 0.22; test 2, r = 0.26).
A number of regressions were then computed, with both test total scores as the criterion variable. In the first set age, gender, having taken a psychology course and self-assessed common sense was entered as predictor variables. The regression was significant for both tests: test 1: (F (4, 219) = 3.40, p < 0.01; Adj R2 = 0.04) and test 2: (F (4, 219) = 4.57, p < 0.001; Adj R2 = 0.06). In both cases only self-assessed common sense was significant Test 1 Beta = .22, t = 3.37, p < .001; Test 2 Beta 0.26, t = 4.05, p < 0.001). Those with higher self-assessed common sense were indeed more able to detect myths. In this sense participants were indeed self-aware.
The two scores were then combined to produce a total score which served as a criterion variable. There were eight predictor variables: age, gender, education, having taken a psychology course, self-assessed common sense, having children, religious beliefs and political beliefs. This was significant (F (8, 211) = 4.50, p < 0.001, Adj R2 = 0.11). Two variables were significant predictors: Common Sense (Beta = 0.21, t = 3.13, p < 0.01) and religious beliefs (Beta = 0.18, t = 2.47, p < 0.01). Those with higher self-rated common sense and greater religious beliefs were better able to detect these myths.
4. Discussion
The current results replicated findings of earlier studies on misconception prevalence (Arntzen, et al., 2010; Furnham & Hughes, 2014) showing that a large number of myths were widely believed. Overall, it seemed that participants were better at identifying Brain Myths compared to Child Developmental myths, though this may be for methodological reasons. Many researchers have categorized myths into various topics like learning and neuromyths (De Bruyckere et al., 2015) though the research tends to show that participants are not particularly better informed and better “myth-detectors” in some areas of study rather than others.
It should be pointed out that when authors list myths of various kinds such as in the book series Great Myths in Psychology commissioned by Wiley they do not indicate the extent to which they believe (or have any data) on the extent to which people would endorse the myths. Nor do they write the statements in such a way as they may easily be understood without reading the subsequent text. In this sense the myths are not all equally clear and easily understood which is the finding of this and other studies.
One interesting finding that was similar to the study of Furnham and Hughes (2014) was the fact that around 15% - 20% answered Don’ Know to the questions. This number varied question by question but on average less than a fifth of respondents were prepared to admit that they did not know. We calculated the total scores for individuals on Don’t Know but this was not systematically related to any of the demographic variables. It could be that people were too embarrassed to admit they did not know when indeed the evidence shows quite clearly that they did not.
This, like other studies in the area, failed to find any strong, clear or logical demographic correlates of myth accepting or rejecting. Age, gender and education were not related to total correct scores, nor education in psychology. Some, but not all, previous studies did establish a small but significant relationship between education in psychology and myth recognition (Furnham & Hughes, 2014) . This study found no such relationship, however this may be because of the lack of detail about that education. The question was simply “Did you ever take a course in psychology/psychiatry: Yes … No …”. As a result, we did not have details about the nature, depth and duration of the course, nor when and where it was undertaken. However, previous studies that collected much more detailed data about general and psychological education also showed, surprisingly, very few significant links.
Both the correlational and regression results did indicate one individual difference marker of myth detection. Those who rated themselves higher on “having common sense” were better detectors of myths. Descartes observed that “common sense is the most widely distributed human characteristic because everybody believes they have a great deal of it” which was reflected in the strong skew on this dimension. Thus, whereas most people agreed to this statement it nevertheless proved a significant correlate of myth detection. There was also some indication that religious and political beliefs were linked to myth detection though the correlations were low. The results were rather surprising showing that more religious and right-wing people tended to be better at myth detection. Some studies have suggested that together political and religious beliefs assess a participants’ ideology. This interesting finding requires both replication and explanation.
The current study has shown that psychological myths and misconceptions (about the brain and child development) are abundant and persistent as well as potentially harmful and socially divisive. It is possible that myth-debunking campaigns designed around reputational methods akin to those used by Kowalski and Taylor (2009), Lilienfeld et al., (2010), and LaCaille (2015) have the potential to reduce levels of misconception. The current study can be used to identify myths and misconceptions in need of refutation.
Like all others this study had limitations. It used a modestly sized convenience rather than a population sample. Further, the test was relative long and it may have meant that some respondents got into a particular response mindset (Furnham et al., 2015) . However, a preliminary “data clean-up” suggested that very few (around 15%) of participants were eliminated for their erratic, incomplete or patterned response set. Also, some of the wording of the statements required some clarification. Further, all the correct answers were false, as in all other studies in this area which may have induced certain response biases. Indeed, this paper may be considered as a pilot for a second study where a reduced number of myths were chosen based on the result of this study perhaps concentrating on those where most participants believed them to be true.
5. Conclusion
Studies such as this provide useful historical data on psychological myth prevalent in society. They nearly always provide the “shocking truth” about the widespread acceptance of myths which nearly always concern experts and educationists who call for attempt to dispel or debunk those myths. The question of how to combat these myths and ensure that people are better informed about various areas of psychology remains largely unanswered.