Developing Reading Automaticity and Fluency: Revisiting What Reading Teachers Know, Putting Confirmed Research into Current Practice ()
1. Introduction
The purposes of this article are to: a) review the seminal literature to define reading automaticity and reading fluency, noting the differences between the concepts; b) explore repeat reading theories and repeat reading teaching practices to help children develop reading fluency; c) contrast and compare the terms of rapid word recognition and automatic word decoding; and d) show how teachers of beginning reading can put accepted past research findings into current classroom practice. Literature and case scenarios show repeat reading practices may be useful for helping children develop rapid word recognition and fluent reading; some repeat reading methods fail to adequately help children develop needed automatic word decoding skills. The article offers a theoretical model of how the reading materials presented to children play a critical role in the children’s development of decoding automaticity, reading fluency, and comprehension of complex text. The final section explores how childhood teaching and learning theories support the theoretical model; both the model and childhood learning theories should be a part of reading teacher education.
2. Defining Terms and Clarifying Assumptions
This article is written within the context of the beginning reader, and beginning reading instruction. For the purposes of this article the beginning reader means young children experiencing the first, formal reading lessons. The beginning reader refers to children ages four through seven.
2.1. Defining Reading
Reading is defined as the ability to look at print, respond with the proper sound translation and comprehend the meaning of the print (Kostewicz & Kibina, 2010; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000) . Reading is a complex skill of constructing meaning from written text; the reader must be able to decode words quickly and accurately so the mind is free to comprehend the text (Kuhn, Schwanenflugel, & Meisinger, 2010) . This essential, rapid decoding skill proficient readers have is called automaticity (Adams, 1990; Kuhn et al., 2010; La Berge & Samuels, 1974; Logan, 1997) .
2.2. Defining Decoding
Decoding is defined as the ability to look at print and respond with the proper sound translation; decoding is a print-to-sound process (Adams, 1990; De Graaff, Bosman, Hasselman, & Verhoeven, 2009) . Decoding refers to the ability to attend to letter sound translations, and spelling patterns to decipher a word. This definition of decoding follows research from Castles & Nation (2008) , Ehri (2005) , Hoien-Tengesdal and Tonnessen (2011) , and Veenendaal, Groen, and Verhoeven (2015) who describe decoding as the ability to connect letters and spelling-patterns to the proper sound translation.
2.3. Defining Decodable Text
For the purposes of this article, the term decodable text refers to simple, regular spelling patterns with short vowel sounds. Examples of decodable text are the consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) pattern, /a/, /o/, /i/, /u/, and, /e/ words: hat, mop, wig, fun, and men; about 250 words, only one spelling pattern (Appendix). Examples of other decodable text are the CVCC pattern: pass, doll, puff, kiss, bell, …camp, damp, lamp…belt, felt, melt… The CCVC pattern: skid, skin, skip, stop, step, snip, spin, spot, swam swim… The CCVCC pattern: black, crack, speck, brick, click, stick, trick, block, clock, stuck, truck... This definition of decodable text aligns with research from Adams (1990) , Denton and Otaiba (2011) , and Greaney and Arrow (2012) .
2.4. Defining Automaticity
Automaticity is defined as the ability of a reader to decode print instantly without conscious thought or effort (Kuhn et al., 2010; La Berge & Samuels, 1974; Logan, 1997) . Automaticity is linked to reading comprehension; when a reader does not have to consciously think about decoding, the reader’s mind is free to comprehend text (Adams, 1990; Kostewicz & Kibina, 2010; Kuhn et al., 2010; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000) . One root cause of poor reading comprehension is lack of automatic decoding skill (Adams, 1990; Kuhn et al., 2010; La Berge & Samuels, 1974; Logan, 1997) . A main goal of reading instruction is to help children acquire automatic decoding ability (Deeney, 2010; De Graaff et al., 2009) .
2.5. Defining Fluency
Fluency is defined as fast, accurate oral reading with proper expression (Kuhn et al., 2010; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Rasinski, 2012; Schwanenflugel et al., 2006) . Beginning reading teachers check for reading automaticity by monitoring reading fluency, often focusing on speed of words read correctly per minute (wcpm) (Deeney, 2010; Guerin & Murphy, 2015; Rasinski, 2012) . Developing reading fluency is a national Common Core Standard of teaching practice (Common Core State Standards Initiative, CCSSI, 2015; Common Core State Standards, CCSS, 2015) .
As with reading automaticity, reading fluency is tied to reading comprehension (Guerin & Murphy, 2015) . Studies also show a connection between proper oral expressive reading (prosody) and improved reading comprehension (Keyes, Cartledge, Gibson, Lenwood, & Robinson-Ervin, 2016; Paige, Rasinski, & Magpuri-Lavell, 2012; Veenendaal et al., 2015) .
2.6. The Merging of Two Concepts, Automaticity and Fluency
The concept of developing reading fluency began to merge with the concept of reading automaticity (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000: p. 3-7) . At times, the terms reading fluency and reading automaticity have merged in the literature; Rasinski (2012: p. 518) writes, “Because fluency (automaticity) has come to be measured by a reader’s speed of reading…”
Researchers agree, gaining automatic decoding skills helps lead to fluent oral reading (Adams, 1990; Kuhn et al., 2010; Samuels, 1979) . The rub is, should the beginning reading instructor strive for oral reading fluency during beginning reading attempts. The following scenario shows instead, the teacher focusing on independent, accurate reading, with comprehension.
2.7. Case Scenario
Nelly is a four-year-old who would not talk. Teachers are still able to teach Nelly to read. First, Nelly would listen to a teacher verbalize a single letter-sound, Nelly would point to the corresponding, lower case letter. Because Nelly would not talk, the option of the teacher pointing to a letter and Nelly responding verbally with the basic sound translation was omitted. Instead, after Nelly could successfully point to 9 letters the teacher verbally “sounded”, Nelly was ushered into reading lessons. Nelly would sit at a table with her teacher. On the table were little toys set up in a row: a rat, a man, a mat, a van, a can, a cat, a hat. The teacher would offer a word on a large strip of paper: van. Nelly had to read the word, and select the item the word represented. Thus, there was silent reading, independent reading, and documentation of reading comprehension. The teacher has no idea if Nelly’s reading is fluent.
When children are first learning to read, the speed of oral reading is individualized (Wolf, 1998; 2014) . Veenendaal et al. (2015) note when children are learning to read, the reading is not fluent. Further, reading teachers should not expect first reading attempts to be fluent because children are learning to decode (Veenendaal et al., 2015) . Yet children must eventually develop reading fluency (Veenendaal et al., 2015) .
3. Repeat Reading to Develop Reading Fluency
A main strategy to help young readers develop fluent reading is repeat reading (Deeney, 2010; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000) . Repeat reading is a strategy in which a child reads the same text over and over until the oral reading becomes fluent (Deeney, 2010; Hicks, 2009; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Samuels, 1979; 1985) . This section explores the repeat reading theories of Jay Samuels, and Carol Chomsky along with other repeat reading research. Next, the section explores repeat reading teaching practices to help children develop reading fluency.
3.1. Samuels’ Repeat Reading Theory and Assumptions
Some repeat reading teaching strategies can be traced back to Samuels’ (1985) seminal writings. Samuels theorized if children repeat read the same text to fluency the children will gain automatic decoding skills (Samuels, 1979; 1985; Samuels & Flor, 1997; Samuels, Schermer, & Reinking, 1992) . Samuels (1979; 1985) asserted repeat reading is a process of practicing rapid decoding. Samuels wrote, “We assume that, because of the extensive practice on rapid recognition of these words, the words are decoded automatically” (Samuels, 1985: p. 228) . The assumption is rapid word recognition involves the same mental processing as automatic word decoding.
3.2. Chomsky’s Repeat Reading Theory and Assumptions
Carol Chomsky at Berkeley was also using repeat reading methods (Chomsky, 1976) . Chomsky surmised repeat reading increased overall reading success, yet reported the repeat reading technique helps children who cannot decode, memorize text (Chomsky, 1976) . Unlike Samuels, Chomsky did not deduce that repeat reading techniques help develop children’s decoding skills, but rather repeat reading helps children memorize text and rapidly recognize words to develop a sense of decoding success (Chomsky, 1976) .
Some beginning reading teachers agree with Chomsky’s theory, that repeat reading the same text to fluency helps children memorize text (A. Zaichenko, personal communication, October 11, 2018). A first grade teacher reports, “Children can repeat read a paragraph to rapid, accurate, and expressive, fluent reading. Often, we teachers can take a simple word out of the paragraph’s context, like the word man, and the children have no idea how to read the word.” The teacher goes on to explain, “The children are learning to memorize a specific text; the children are not learning to read” (A. Zaichenko, personal communication, October 11, 2018).
3.3. Repeat Reading Research
The four repeat reading teaching strategies are: a) children echo-read after a teacher, b) children read with a tape-recorded voice reading the text, c) children choral read with entire class, and d) children repeat read text independently (Faulkner & Levy, 1999; Homan, Klesius, & Hite, 1993; Labbo & Teale, 1990; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Sindelar, Monda, & O’Shea, 1990) .
A majority of early studies found repeat reading the same text to fluency helped children improve decoding, and ultimately improve children’s reading comprehension of the specific text repeatedly read (Faulkner & Levy, 1999; Homan et al., 1993; Labbo & Teale, 1990; O’Shea, Sindelar, & O’Shea, 1985; Sindelar et al., 1990) . Most initial repeat reading studies did not attempt to measure decoding transfer skills to new text (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000: p. 3-15) .
Other studies found, although repeat reading techniques may improve children’s reading fluency of the repeated text; repeat reading interventions did not correlate to improved reading comprehension (Deeney, 2010; Fleisher, Jenkins, & Pany, 1979; Hicks, 2009; Kuhn, 2005; Samuels, 1985; Therrien & Hughes, 2008; Valencia et al., 2010) . Some studies found repeat reading using different text increases reading comprehension more than repeat reading the same text (Kuhn, 2005; Therrien & Hughes, 2008) .
Today repeat reading continues to be a strategy used in classrooms with the purpose of developing children’s automatic word decoding skill (Guerin & Murphy, 2015) . However, in the past, the term automatic word decoding has been interchanged with the term rapid word recognition (Samuels, 1985) . Accordingly, there needs to be a close examination of the terms rapid word recognition, and automatic word decoding.
4. Rapid Word Recognition and Automatic Word Decoding
There is agreement that beginning readers need opportunities to decode text so the skill can become automatic (Adams, 1990; Cohen & Brady, 2011; Denton & Otaiba, 2011; Hoien-Tengesdal & Tonnessen, 2011; Mc Candliss, Beck, Sandak, & Perfetti, 2003; Samuels, 1985) . However, beginning readers will often use non-decoding strategies to rapidly recognize words (Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985) . The following case scenarios illustrate how children use many different strategies to read words.
1) Case Scenario
Mark is five. He looks at the word mom and says, “Mom. I know that word, ‘m’ ‘o’ ‘m’ (em-oh-em), mom.” Mark fluently verbalizes the word mom; he spells and says the word mom. Mark has no idea “m” has a sound translation of /m/. Mark demonstrates rapid word recognition of mom. Mark does not automatically decode the word, mom.
2) Case Scenario
Joan is six. She fluent reads a passage from a Dr. Seuss book, “I do not like green eggs and ham. I do not like them Sam I Am.” The teacher points to the word green and asks,
“How do you know this word?”
Joan points to the word green and replies, “I know this is green because green is the longest word, and see the beginning…,” Joan points to the “g”, “It [the g]hangs down like a hook.” Joan goes on to explain her rapid word recognition strategies pointing to the word eggs, “I know this is eggs, see? The two hooks (points to the gg letters) are at the end.”
The assumption that rapid word recognition is the same mental process as automatic word decoding is incorrect. Children can rapidly recognize words using many types of strategies. Rapid word recognition often has nothing to do with the ability to decode words or decode spelling patterns (Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985; Wolf, 2016) . Some reading materials offered to beginning readers facilitate, not decoding, but rapid word recognition strategies.
3) Case Scenario
Diane is six, in first grade. She uses a beginning reading book that encourages her to look at pictures to recognize words. Diane fluently reads, “The tree is green. The ball is red.”
The teacher explains, “These are sentence-pattern books. The children name an object then name the color of the object. The children tend to pay more attention to the pictures than the print” (A. Zaichenko, personal communication, October 11, 2018).
4) Case Scenario
This case details the differences between rapid word recognition and automatic word decoding. Tom is in first grade. Tom is falling behind in his reading development. The reading teacher checks to see if Tom can, not name lower case letter forms, but instead, if Tom can respond with the basic sound-translation or “read” the 26 letters. Tom can “read” each letter.
Next, the reading teacher checks to see if Tom can read the basic consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) spelling pattern of /a/ words, such as, ran, tan, van, map, cat, gas, pal, ham―60 words. Tom does quite well. The teacher helps Tom with some words, modeling the blending of sounds into words. Tom’s reading is not fluent. During this initial visit, the reading teacher has Tom read /o/ words, such as mom, log, Tom… The /o/CVC words seem harder for Tom, in fact he is unable to decode his own name, Tom. Tom, asks for help. The reading teacher says, “I am not going to help you read this word.” “Take the /o/ book home Tom,” the teacher says, “Call me up when you read the word.” About 30 minutes later the teacher’s phone rings. It is Tom.
“It’s my name, Tom, I read my name!” Tom is excited. Tom’s mother gets on the phone and says, “Tom has been recognizing his name for years, on birthday cards, Christmas presents―this is the first time he has ever read his name.”
These case scenarios demonstrate the different strategies children use to read. Beginning reading teachers must concern themselves with the types of strategies children are encouraged to use when first learning to read. When learning to read, children will utilize the reading strategies they are exposed to first (Castles & Nation, 2008; Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985; Veenendaal et al., 2015) . Unless taught to decode words from the beginning of instruction, children will use other strategies to recognize words (Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985) . A first grade teacher describes a chilling reality:
“Children can learn to read many words by sight. But if somewhere along the way they do not learn to decode, they will not learn to read. It’s these kids who get held back, and these same kids who drop out of school. You get so you can predict which ones it’s going to be.” ( Wolf, 1998: p. 17 , M. Nicholson, personal communication, March 20, 1998).
Years of known data confirms this teacher’s observations. To prevent later reading difficulties, early reading instruction should focus on children practicing decoding (Mc Candliss et al., 2003) . Children who exhibit poor foundational decoding skills are often the children who: a) develop poor reading comprehension (Kuhn et al., 2010) , b) drop out of school (Denton & Otaiba, 2011) , and c) have lasting, poor literacy, thus academic struggles (Adams, 1990; Denton & Otaiba, 2011; Juel, 1988) .
5. Why Text Matters: Improving Repeat Reading Teaching Strategies
It is important for children to apply letter sound and spelling pattern knowledge as a primary reading strategy (Beck, 1998; Chard & Osborn, 1999; Castles & Nation, 2008; Mc Candliss et al., 2003) . Further, the type of text children try to read shapes the reading strategies children use (Castles & Nation, 2008; Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985; Veenendaal et al., 2015) . Accordingly, the type of text beginning readers are asked to repeat read will determine the type of reading strategies children practice and develop. The following case scenarios demonstrate how beginning reading materials will foster different reading strategies.
1) Case Scenario
Hannah’s first reading lessons involve reading pattern sentences, I like to ____. Hannah fills in the blank and is able to fluently read, “I like to eat ice cream! I like to go swimming!”Hannah is reading many different spelling patterns. Text memorization, and sight word recognition are the initial reading strategies Hannah learns. When the teacher shows Hannah the word “to” out of the sentence pattern, Hannah is unable to read the word.
2) Case Scenario
Sometimes children learning to read will revert to looking at a picture and thus guess what a word “says”. Luis is four. He reads “Dan ran to the… ice cream truck?” Luis is looking at the picture of an ice cream truck on the page. Luis looks up questioningly at the teacher. The teacher silently prompts with a finger-tap on the last word. Luis self-corrects, “van”. Luis repeat reads, “Dan ran to the van!” This time when re-reading Luis’s gaze remains on the text and there is an excited confidence in his voice. Luis reads by a) decoding CVC words, b) looking at a picture, c) guessing, and finally d) self-correcting re-using decoding skills. Luis wants to continue reading, and when given a choice, Luis chooses the decodable book to take home and read to his parents. “I can read this book!” Luis tells his parents. Luis seems motivated to read the decodable book because, a) reading is not viewed as an endless memorization, or guessing process, b) Luis is using his letter-sound knowledge to decode words, and c) Luis is proud that he is independently reading, and comprehending print, with little help from the teacher. Luis’s reading is slow, and not fluent. Luis is having to attend to every letter in every word. Luis is slowly building decoding skills, not by repeat reading the same text, but by reading slightly different, and simple, decodable text.
6. Developing Automaticity and Reading Fluency: A Theoretical Model
There are two pathways to fluent reading 1) rapid word recognition, and 2) automatic word decoding. During beginning reading instruction, teaching strategies and text(s) should aim to develop automatic word decoding skills. Children will apply decoding skills if beginning reading text offers simple, decodable spelling patterns (Morris, 2015) . The below model illustrates the two pathways beginning readers can take to develop fluent reading (Figure 1).
Rapid word recognition, sometimes called sight word recognition (Adams, 1990; Walton & Walton, 2002) is initially a quick path to reading fluency. Repeat reading the exact same text develops rapid word recognition through the
![]()
Figure 1. A theoretical model: beginning readers have two pathways to fluent reading.
pathway of text memorization. An example of text children repeat read is: There is no school today. Mother Cat has many little helpers. What a busy house! What a noisy house! (Scarry, 1986: p. 1) .
In contrast, when reading slightly different, decodable text children must practice word decoding. An example of slightly different decodable text is: Nan ran. Nan ran to the van. Nan ran to the tan van. (Rasmussen & Goldberg, 1985: p. 8) . When children practice decoding, children take longer to reach effortless, automatic decoding, and reading fluency (Veenendaal et al., 2015) . In the long run, fluency that arises from the roots of automatic decoding is what beginning readers must develop.
Adams’ review of research confirms how proficient readers process “virtually” every individual letter of every word they read (Adams, 1990: p. 410) . Beginning reading materials should help young readers practice the same actions of the skillful reader (Wolf, 2016) . Teachers should not offer children text that is easily memorized. Teachers should offer young children slightly different, decodable text that ensures children’s very first reading attempts develop the skill of processing every letter of every word.
7. Use of Decodable Text: Research and Teaching Implications
Currently the national Common Core standards recommend beginning reading materials be drawn from acclaimed children’s literature in areas of adventure stories, folktales, legends, fables, fantasy, realistic fiction, and myth (Common Core State Standards, 2015) . The titles of recommended beginning reading materials offer a preview of the spelling patterns and text children are asked to decode: Over in the Meadow by John Langstaff, or Pancakes for Breakfast by Tomie De Paola (Common Core State Standards, 2015) . Beautiful literature: not easy text for a beginning reader to apply decoding skills.
In contrast, decodable text for beginning readers may have drawbacks. The initial stories may bore children. One school of thought is decodable text, sentences like: Dan ran. The man ran would stifle children’s motivation to read, or mix children up with such similar spellings (Adams, 1990) . There is no found research to support these claims (Wolf, 1998; 2014) .
There is convincing data that simple, decodable text is ideal for the beginning reader because a) during children’s very first reading attempts, decodable text promotes the use of letter sound knowledge and spelling pattern knowledge as the primary reading strategy, and b) children are usually successful in applying their letter sound knowledge to decode words (Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985) . In addition, it is readers who learn to respond to letter or spelling patterns, often called orthographic reading, who become the best readers in terms of decoding and reading comprehension (Castles & Nation, 2008; Veenendaal et al., 2015) . Verhoeven and Leeuwe’s (2009) seminal 6-year study, with close to 3000 participants, confirmed it was the 5-year-old children who could read the basic CVC spelling pattern who were still the best readers in sixth grade.
Decodable text has the following benefits for children learning to read:
1) From the very first day, the reading lessons are kept as simple and easy as possible ensuring independent reading success in decoding and comprehension;
2) Words belong to the oral language of children so the children will derive meaning as words and sentences are successfully decoded;
3) The words in decodable texts belong to a consistent spelling pattern so the beginning reader has the advantage of learning by spelling pattern instead of by an accumulation of individual words with different spelling patterns;
4) The spelling patterns presented are stepping stones, upon which decoding of succeeding words may be based (top, stop, stops…)
5) Children are using, not rote memory, but their minds to read which sparks a desire to want to read more. That is, learning to read is motivating (Wolf, 1998; 2014) .
Research supports the aforementioned points, a) highlighting spelling-to-sound regularities helps to develop children’s automatic decoding skills (Greaney & Arrow, 2012; Verhoeven & Leeuwe, 2009) , and, b) the activity of learning to read creates motivation to read (Paris & Carpenter, 2004) . Further, the theoretical model of the pathway to automaticity and fluent reading is supported by childhood teaching and learning theories.
8. Reading Teacher Education: Bridging Theory to Classroom Practice
The education of reading teachers should help teachers bridge childhood learning theories into classroom practice (Agbenyega, 2009; National Institute for Literacy, NIFL, 2008; Wolf, 1998) . Derived from Jean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky, and Maria Montessori’s seminal work, there are three childhood learning principles which align with beginning reading instruction (Agbenyega, 2009; Cossentino, 2006; Tzuo, 2007) . These three main principles also align with the theoretical model of the pathway to developing automaticity and fluent reading.
1) Principle One: Provide the Necessary Materials for Learning
The primary role of the teacher is to provide children with the necessary materials so children can easily learn (Montessori, trans. 1965, 1966; Piaget, trans. 1952) . Montessori, and Piaget wrote how true learning and deep understanding are not efforts of memory (Montessori, trans., 1965, 1966; Piaget, trans., 1952) . Instead of using rote-memorization, children must have the necessary materials to be able to use internal mental processing to construct knowledge (Gredler, 2009; Inhelder & Piaget, trans. 1958; Piaget, trans. 1952) . When children are learning to read, the teacher must give children the necessary materials so children can use the mental process of decoding to construct words (Bracken & Crawford, 2010; Montessori, 1965, 1965) . In the case of beginning reading instruction, the necessary materials are decodable text (Greaney & Arrow, 2012; Wolf, 1998, 2016) .
2) Principle Two: Scaffolding Teaching Materials Facilitates Learning
Piaget, Vygotsky, and Montessori all affirmed it is specifically organized, or scaffolded teaching materials which fosters learning in children (Inhelder & Piaget, trans. 1958; Montessori, trans. 1965; trans. 1966; Piaget, trans. 1952; Vygotsky, trans. 1978) . Montessori and Vygotsky claimed when teachers purposely scaffold educational materials, these materials enhance independent learning (Montessori, trans. 1965; trans. 1966; Vygotsky, trans. 1978) . Scaffolding is a process of building learning curriculum, activities, and materials in a systematic order in which mastering each skill, leads to the ability to transfer the knowledge to master a higher order skill (Hmelo-Silver, Duncan, & Chinn, 2007; Montessori, trans. 1965; trans. 1966; Piaget, trans. 1952; Vygotsky, trans. 1978) .
To facilitate learning to read, teachers should learn how to scaffold reading materials (Cohen, & Brady, 2011; Wolf, 1998, 2016) . In the case of beginning reading instruction, the scaffolded materials are slightly different, decodable text. Scaffolding text helps children apply the prior constructed knowledge to construct new patterns of knowledge. The new patterns of knowledge children construct are new spelling patterns.
A valuable educational preparation for beginning reading teachers is to explore scaffolding of a) CVC words (Appendix), b) CVC sentence length, and c) decodable spelling patterns. Reading teachers should be taught how to scaffold simple spelling patterns, to more complex patterns for children to read: CVC pattern: bat, cat, fat… CVCC pattern: pass, doll, puff… CCVC pattern: skin, skip, stop, step, snip, spin, swim… CCVCC pattern: black, crack, brick, click, stick, trick, stuck, truck...A benefit of educating reading teachers on scaffolding spelling patterns, is reading teachers are then equipped to assess the quality of beginning reading programs and materials a school district might want to adopt.
3) Principle Three: Authentic Practice
Important to each childhood learning theorist, Piaget, Vygotsky, and Montessori was how authentic experience and repeated, authentic practice creates learning (Inhelder & Piaget, trans. 1958; Montessori, trans. 1965; trans. 1966; Piaget, trans. 1952; Vygotsky, trans. 1978) . In the process of learning to read, children must have authentic reading experience. Children must practice the print-to-sound decoding processing and comprehension of print. The theoretical model on pathways to reading fluency shows how it is the pathway of decoding simple spelling patterns that provides authentic, repeated practice of reading.
In summary, there are three guiding theories that facilitate children’s learning to read a) provide necessary materials, b) scaffold the reading materials or text(s), and c) create opportunities for authentic reading practice. Teacher education can enable teachers to put these key principles into classroom practice. When put into action, the three childhood learning principles, and the theoretical model, combine to make a powerful beginning reading curriculum.
9. Implications of the Theoretical Model for Future Research
The theoretical model applies confirmed research evidence that beginning reading text should encourage children to pay attention to letter sound translations in order to practice decoding simple words. Further, when the text is slightly different, children must pay attention to every letter in every word, practicing the behaviors of proficient readers. More research is needed on beginning reading text, and scaffolding text. Research is needed on beginning reading teachers’ abilities to assess and evaluate beginning reading text(s).
10. Conclusion
This article reviewed the relationship between the concepts of reading automaticity and reading fluency. Automatic decoding skills are needed for fluent reading, and in turn reading comprehension (Kuhn et al., 2010; Wolf, 2016) . A theoretical model postulates that depending on the type of text, children are asked to read, children will either practice rapid word recognition strategies, or children will practice word decoding strategies. The latter, slower strategy of practicing word decoding with attention to letters and spelling patterns helps children develop decoding automaticity, and thus fluency arising from the roots of automaticity.
The simple, theoretical model is backed by research and childhood learning theories of Piaget, Vygotsky, and Montessori. Education of beginning reading teachers can help bridge these theories into classroom practice. When the beginning reader is not memorizing text and is instead reading by using the mental process of decoding, a spark ignites a desire; children want to read more, learn more.
Appendix
Consonant-Vowel-Consonant (CVC) Basic Spelling Pattern: Scaffolded for Reading Ease.
/a/ book
can bat ham cab bag bad cap gal gas
Dan cat Pam dab gag dad gap Hal
fan fat ram gab hag Dad lap pal
man hat Sam jab lag had map Al
Nan mat yam lab nag lad tap
pan pat am tab rag mad zap
ran rat sag pad
tan sat tag sad
van at wag add
an
/o/ book
mom con cot bob cog cod bop off
Mom Don dot Bob dog god cop
Tom Ron got cob fog God hop
on hot job hog nod mop
jot lob jog pod pop
lot mob log rod sop
not rob odd top
pot Rob
rot sob
/u/ book
bun but bum cub bug bud cup bus
fun cut gum hub dug cud pup Gus
gun gut hum nub hug dud pus
pun hut rum pub jug mud us
run jut sum rub lug
sun nut yum sub mug
nun ut tub rug
/i/ book
fin bit dim bib big bid dip
kin fit him fib dig did hip
pin hit Jim rib fig hid lip
sin kit Kim jig lid nip
tin lit rim pig lid rip
win nit Tim rig Sid sip
in pit wig tip
quit zip
sit
it ill
sis
quiz
/e/ book
Ben bet beg bed Mel
den get keg fed Nel
hen jet leg Jed elf
Ken let Meg led elk
pen net Peg red hem
ten pet Ted pep
set wed web
vet Ed ebb
wet Wes
yet yes