Double-Step Adaptation of Saccadic Eye Movements Is Influenced by Priming with Age Stereotypies

Abstract

Age related deficits of sensorimotor adaptation have been observed earlier with arm, but not with eye movements. Here we evaluate whether deficits of eye adaptation may depend on the subjects’ believes about their own sensorimotor abilities. To find out, elderly subjects were primed with positive or negative age stereotypes using the scrambled-sentence task, and were then exposed to a double-step saccade adaptation task. The outcome was compared to data from an earlier study with unprimed elderly persons. We found adaptation to be stronger after positive priming than after negative or no priming, with no difference between the latter two. Aftereffects of adaptation were not modified by priming. From this we conclude that positive primes enhanced workaround strategies, but not adaptive recalibration, while negative primes failed completely, possibly because of a floor effect.

Share and Cite:

Bock, O. , Grigorova, V. and Ilieva, M. (2013) Double-Step Adaptation of Saccadic Eye Movements Is Influenced by Priming with Age Stereotypies. Psychology, 4, 1014-1017. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.412147.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Abrams, D., Vauclair, C.-M., & Swift, H. (2011). Predictors of attitudes to age across Europe. London: Department for Work and Pensions.
[2] Ball, K., & Owsley, C. (1993). The useful field of view test: A new technique for evaluating age-related declines in visual function. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 64, 71-79.
[3] Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230-244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
[4] Becker, W., & Jürgens, R. (1979). An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research, 19, 967-983.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(79)90222-0
[5] Beurskens, R., & Bock, O. (2012). Age-related decline of peripheral visual processing: The role of eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 217, 117-124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2978-3
[6] Bock, O. (2005). Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 259-263.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2133-5
[7] Bock, O. (2013). Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: A review and synthesis of behavioral findings. Frontiers Hum Neuroscience, 7, 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00081
[8] Bock, O., & Girgenrath, M. (2006). Relationship between sensorimotor adaptation and cognitive functions in younger and older subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 169, 400-406.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0153-4
[9] Bock, O., Schmitz, G., & Grigorova, V. (2008). Transfer of adaptation between ocular saccades and arm movements. Human Movement Science, 27, 383-395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2008.01.001
[10] Bono, F., Oliveri, R., Zappia, M., Aguglia, U., Puccio, G., & Quattrone, A. (1996). Computerized analysis of eye movements as a function of age. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 22, 261-269.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-4943(96)00698-X
[11] Chapman, G. J., & Hollands, M. A. (2006). Evidence for a link between changes to gaze behaviour and risk of falling in older adults during adaptive locomotion. Gait & Posture, 24, 288-294.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.10.002
[12] Fernández-Ruiz, J., Hall, C., Vergara, P., & Díaz, R. (2000). Prism adaptation in normal aging: Slower adaptation rate and larger aftereffect. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 223-226.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00057-9
[13] Grigorova, V., Bock, O., & Borisova, S. (2013). Concurrent adaptation of reactive saccades and hand pointing movements to equal and to opposite changes of target direction. Experimental Brain Research, 226, 63-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3411-x
[14] Hart, W., & Albarracín, D. (2009). The effects of chronic achievement motivation and achievement primes on the activation of achievement and fun goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1129-1141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017146
[15] Hausdorff, J. M., Levy, B. R., & Wei, J. Y. (1999). The power of ageism on physical functions of older persons: Reversibility of age-related gait changes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47, 1346-1349.
[16] Hüttermann, S., Memmert, D., & Bock, O. (2012). Semantic priming of attention focus: Evidence for short- and long-term effects. Psychology, 3, 128-131. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2012.32019
[17] Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., & Hoffmann, J. (2007). Mechanisms of subliminal response priming. Advanced Cognitive Psychology, 3, 307-315. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0032-1
[18] Levy, B. (2000). Handwriting as a reflection of aging self-stereotypes. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33, 81-94.
[19] Levy, B. R., Hausdorff, J. M., Hencke, R., & Wei, J. Y. (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 55, 205-213.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205
[20] Levy, B. R., & Leifheit-Limson, E. (2009). The stereotype-matching effect: Greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes. Psychology and Aging, 24, 230-233.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014563
[21] Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: successful memory in China and among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 989-997.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.989
[22] Maltz, M., & Shinar, D. (1999). Eye movements of younger and older drivers. Human Factors, 41, 15-25.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1518/001872099779577282
[23] Mayer, J., & Mussweiler, T. (2011). Suspicious spirits, flexible minds: When distrust enhances creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1262-1277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024407
[24] McNay, E., & Willingham, D. (1998). Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptualmotor recalibration, with aging. Learning Memory, 4, 411-420.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.4.5.411
[25] Paquette, C., & Fung, J. (2011). Old age affects gaze and postural coordination. Gait & Posture, 33, 227-232.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.11.010
[26] Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., & Gobancé, L. (2009). Subliminal priming of motivational orientation in educational settings: Effect on academic performance moderated by mindfulness. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 695-698.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.011
[27] Redding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1996). Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 379-394.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.379
[28] Sekuler, R., & Ball, K. (1986). Visual localization: Age and practice. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 3, 864-867.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.3.000864
[29] Shah, A., Bhat, R., McKenzie, S., & Koen, C. (2007). Elderly suicide rates: Cross-national comparisons and association with sex and elderly age-bands. Medicine, Science and the Law, 47, 244-252.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/rsmmsl.47.3.244
[30] Strahan, E., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2002). Subliminal priming and persuasion: Striking while the iron is hot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 556-568.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00502-4

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.