[1]
|
Asch, S. E., & Witkin, H. A. (1948). Studies in space orientation: II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 455-477.
doi:10.1037/h0054121
|
[2]
|
Behrmann, M., & Tipper, S. P. (1999). Attention accesses multiple reference frames: Evidence from visual neglect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 83-101.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.83
|
[3]
|
Blackmore, C., & Carpenter, R. H., & Georgeson, M. A. (1970). Lateral inhibition between orientation detectors in the human visual system. Nature, 228, 37-39. doi:10.1038/228037a0
|
[4]
|
Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Logan, G. D. (1997). The influence of reference frame selection on spatial template construction. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 411-437. doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2519
|
[5]
|
Carpenter, R. H. S., & Blakemore, C. (1973). Interactions between orientations in human vision. Experimental Brain Research, 18, 287-303.
doi:10.1007/BF00234599
|
[6]
|
Coren, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1978). Seeing is deceiving: The psychology of visual illusions. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
|
[7]
|
Day, R. H., & Dickinson, R. G. (1976). Apparent length of the arms of acute and obtuse angles, and the components of the Muller-Lyer illusion. Australian Journal of Psychology, 28, 137-148.
doi:10.1080/00049537608254638
|
[8]
|
Day, R. H., & Jolly, W. J. (1987). A note on apparent displacement of lines and dots on oblique parallels. Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 187-189.
doi:10.3758/BF03204880
|
[9]
|
Day, R. H., Watson, W. L., & Jolly, W. J. (1986). The Poggendorff displacement effect with only three dots. Perception & Psychophysics, 39, 351-354.
doi:10.3758/BF03203003
|
[10]
|
De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1990). Spatial vision. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
|
[11]
|
De Valois, K. K., De Valois, R. L., & Yund, E. W. (1979). Responses of striate cortex cells to grating and checkerboard patterns. The Journal of Physiology, 291, 483-505.
|
[12]
|
DiLorenzo, J., & Rock, I. (1982). The rod and frame effect as a function of the righting of the frame. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 536-546.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.536
|
[13]
|
Eagleman, D. M. (2001). Visual illusions and neurobiology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 920-926. doi:10.1038/35104092
|
[14]
|
Ernst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal way. Nature, 415, 429-433.
doi:10.1038/415429a
|
[15]
|
Fineman, M. (1996) Poggendorff’s illusion. In M. Fineman (Ed.), The nature of visual illusion (pp. 151-159). New York: Dover.
|
[16]
|
Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2005). Examining the crossmodal consequences of viewing the Müller-Lyer illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 162, 490-496.
doi:10.1007/s00221-004-2130-8
|
[17]
|
Gibson, J. J. (1937). Adaptation, after effect and contrast in the perception of the tilted lines: II. Simultaneous contrast and the areal restriction of the after effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 553-569.
doi:10.1037/h0057585
|
[18]
|
Gibson, J. J., & Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, aftereffect and contrast in the perception of the tilted lines: I. Quantitative studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 453-467. doi:10.1037/h0059826
|
[19]
|
Gregory, R. L. (1968). Visual illusions. Scientific American, 219, 66-76.
doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1168-66
|
[20]
|
Hering, E. (1861). Beitrage zur psychologie. Leipzig: Engleman.
|
[21]
|
Judd, C. H. (1902). Practice and its effect on the perception of illusions. Psychological Review, 9, 27-39. doi:10.1037/h0073071
|
[22]
|
Kanizsa, G. (1972). Schrumpfung von visuellen Feldern bei amodaler Erganzung. Studia Psychologica, 14, 208-210.
|
[23]
|
Kanizsa, G. (1975). Amodal completion and phenomenal shrinkage of surfaces in the visual field. Italian Journal of Psychology, 2, 187-195.
|
[24]
|
Masini, R., Costa, T., Ferraro, M., & De Marco, A. (1994). Modifications of the Poggendorff effect as a function of random dot textures between the verticals. Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 505-512.
doi:10.3758/BF03205308
|
[25]
|
Matin, L., & Fox, C. R. (1989). Visually perceived eye level and perceived elevation of objects: Linerly additive influences from visual field pitch and from gravity. Vision Research, 29, 315-324.
doi:10.1016/0042-6989(89)90080-1
|
[26]
|
Millar, S., & Al-Attar, Z. (2002). The Müller-Lyer illusion in touch and vision: Implications for multisensory processes. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 353-365. doi:10.3758/BF03194709
|
[27]
|
Parks, T. E. (2009). Demystifying the Poggendorff: A start. Perception, 38, 142-144.
doi:10.1068/p5512
|
[28]
|
Porac, C. (1994). Comparison of the wings-in, wings-out, and Brentano variants of the Müller-Lyer illusion. American Journal of Psychology, 107, 69-83.
doi:10.2307/1423290
|
[29]
|
Predebon, J. (2001). Spatial range of illusory effects in Müller-Lyer figures. Psychological Research, 65, 226-234.
doi:10.1007/s004260100066
|
[30]
|
Pressey, A. W., & Sweeney, O. (1972). Acute angles and the Poggendorff illusion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 169-174.
doi:10.1080/00335557243000049
|
[31]
|
Pressey, A. W., & Di Lollo, V. (1978). Effects of distance between standard and comparison lines on the Muller-Lyer Illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 24, 415-419. doi:10.3758/BF03199738
|
[32]
|
Prinzmetal, W., Shimamura, A. P., & Mikolinski, M. (2001). The Ponzo illusion and the Tilt-Constancy theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 99-114. doi:10.3758/BF03200506
|
[33]
|
Prinzmetal, W., & Beck, D. M. (2001). The tilt constancy theory of visual illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 206-217.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.206
|
[34]
|
Purves, D., & Lotto, B. (2002). Why we see what we do: An empirical theory of vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
|
[35]
|
Rock, I. (1990). The frame of reference. In R. Irvin (Ed.), The legacy of Solomon Asch: Essays in cognition and social psychology (pp. 243-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
|
[36]
|
Spence, C., & Driver J. (Eds.) (2004). Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
|
[37]
|
Weintraub, D., Krantz, D., & Olson, T. (1980). The Poggendorff illusion: Consider all angles. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 718-725.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.718
|
[38]
|
Wenderoth, P. (1974). The distinction between the rod-and-frame illusion and the rod-and-frame test. Perception, 3, 205-212.
doi:10.1068/p030205
|
[39]
|
Wenderoth, P. (1977). An analysis of the rod-and-frame illusion and its variants, In R. H. Day, & G. V. Stanley (Eds.), Studies in perception. Perth: University of Western Australia Press.
|
[40]
|
Wenderoth, P., & Beh, H. (1977). Component analysis of orientation illusions. Perception, 6, 57-75.
doi:10.1068/p060057
|
[41]
|
Wenderoth, P., & van der Zwan, R. (1991). Local and global mechanisms of one- and two-dimensional orientation illusions. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 321-332. doi:10.3758/BF03212224
|
[42]
|
Wilson, A. E., & Pressey, A. W. (1976). The role of apparent distance in the Poggendoff illusion. Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 309-316.
doi:10.3758/BF03199458
|
[43]
|
Zanuttini, L. (1973). Una nuova spiegazione dell'illusione di Poggendorff. (A new explanation for the Poggendorff illusion). Rivista di Psicologia, 67, 295-314.
|
[44]
|
Zanuttini, L. (1976). A new explanation for the Poggendorff illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 20, 29-32. doi:10.3758/BF03198700
|
[45]
|
Zoccolotti, P., Antonucci, G., & Spinelli, D. (1993). The gap between the rod and the frame influences the rod-and-frame effect with small and large inducing displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 4, 14-19.
|