[1]
|
Candel, I., Memon, A., & Al-Harazi, F. (2007). Peer discussion affects children’s memory reports. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1191- 1199. doi:10.1002/acp.1355
|
[2]
|
Davies, G., & Pezdek, K. (2010). Children as witnesses. In G. J. Towl, & D. A. Crighton (Eds.), Forensic Psychology (pp. 178-193). Leicester, UK: BPS Blackwell.
|
[3]
|
French, L., Garry, M., & Mori, K. (2008). You say tomato? Collaborative remembering between intimate couples leads to more false memories than for strangers. Memory, 16, 262-273.
doi:10.1080/09658210701801491
|
[4]
|
French, L., Garry, M., & Mori, K. (2011). Relative—Not absolute— Judgments of credibility affect susceptibility to misinformation conveyed during discussion. Acta Psychologica, 136, 119-128.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.009
|
[5]
|
French, L., Gerrie, M. P., Garry, M., & Mori, K. (2009). Evidence for the efficacy of the MORI technique: Viewers do not notice or implicitly remember details from the alternate movie version. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1224-1232. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1224
|
[6]
|
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memories for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533-543. doi:10.1002/acp.885
|
[7]
|
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Wright, D. B. (2007). I saw it for longer than you: The relationship between perceived encoding duration and memory conformity. Acta Psychologica, 124, 319-331.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.03.009
|
[8]
|
Hanayama, A. & Mori, K. (2011). Conformity of six-year-old children in the Asch experiment without using confederates. Psychology, 2, 661-664. doi:10.4236/psych.2011.27100
|
[9]
|
Matsuno, E., Mori, K., Hirokawa, K., & Ukita, J. (2005). The effects of gender differences in pairs of eyewitnesses on recall memory. Japanese Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3, 83-94.
doi:10.5265/jcogpsy.3.83
|
[10]
|
Kanematsu, H., Mori, K., & Mori, H. (1996/2003). Kotonaru Jitai wo Mokugekishita Futari no Mokugekisha no Hanashiai niyoru Kioku no Hen-you [Memory distortion in eyewitness pairs who observed nonconforming events and discussed them]. Cognitive Studies: Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society, 3, 29-40, 1996. (In Japanese. English version published in Journal of the Faculty of Education, Shinshu University, 109, 75-84, 2003).
|
[11]
|
Mori, K. (2003). Surreptitiously projecting different movies to two subsets of viewers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 599-604. doi:10.3758/BF03195539
|
[12]
|
Mori, K. (2007). A revised method for projecting two different movies to two groups of viewers without their noticing the duality. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 574-578. doi:10.3758/BF03193028
|
[13]
|
Mori, K., & Arai, M. (2010). No need to fake it: Reproduction of the Asch experiment without using confederates. International Journal of Psychology, 45, 390-397. doi:10.1080/00207591003774485
|
[14]
|
Mori, K., & Kitabayashi, M. (2009). How child-mother pairs reported what they had witnessed together: An experimental examination using the MORI technique. Psychology Journal, 6, 60-69.
|
[15]
|
Mori, K., & Mori, H. (2008). Conformity among cowitnesses sharing same or different information about an event in experimental collaborative eyewitness testimony. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 275-290. doi:10.2466/pms.106.1.275-290
|
[16]
|
Roos af Hjelms?ter, E., Granhag, P. A., Str?mwall, L. A., & Memon, A. (2008). The effects of social influence on children’s memory reports: The omission and commission error asymmetry. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 507-513.
|
[17]
|
Wright, D. B., Memon, A., Skagerberg, E. M., & Gabbert, F. (2009). When eyewitnesses talk. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 174-178. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01631.x
|