American Sign Language, Peer Play, and the Deaf Child: A Case Study of Ann
Millicent Musyoka
Lamar University, Beaumont, USA.
DOI: 10.4236/psych.2015.614178   PDF   HTML   XML   2,807 Downloads   4,551 Views   Citations

Abstract

Play provides signing deaf children with the opportunity to communicate and interact with peers while they use and develop conversation skills or extended discourse in American Sign Language (ASL) (Musyoka, 2015). The goal of this study is to examine how play supports thinking, imagination, social, language and literacy development. To analyze the effectiveness of play, the researcher investigated how one deaf child, Ann used her ASL with various play partners in different play centers set up by the teacher in a preschool classroom that followed the ASL/English bilingual philosophy.

Share and Cite:

Musyoka, M. (2015) American Sign Language, Peer Play, and the Deaf Child: A Case Study of Ann. Psychology, 6, 1822-1831. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.614178.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Allen, T. (1986). Patterns of Academic Achievement among Hearing Impaired Students: 1944 and 1983. In Karchmer (Ed.), Deaf Children in America (pp. 161-206). Boston: Little Brown.
[2] Aukrust, V. G. (2004) Explanatory Discourse in Young Second Language Learners’ Peer Play. Discourse Studies, 6, 393-412.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461445604044296
[3] Brackett, D., & Henniges, M. (1976). Communicative Interaction of Preschool Hearing Impaired children in an integrated setting. The Volta Review, 78, 276-285.
[4] Brown, M., & Bergen, D. (2002). Play and Social Interaction of Children with Disabilities at Learning/Activity Centers in an Inclusive Preschool. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 17, 26-37.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02568540209594996
[5] Carpenter, R., Mastergeorge, A., & Coggins, T. (1983). The Acquisition of Communicative Intentions in Infants Eight to Eighteen Months of Age. Language and Speech, 26, 101-116.Cornelius, G. & Hornett, D. (1990). Play behavior of Hearing-Impaired Kindergarten Children. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 316-321.
[6] Darbyshire, J. (1977). Play Patterns in Young Children with Impaired Hearing. Volta Review, 79, 19-26.
[7] Dickinson, D., & Tabors, P.T. (Eds.) (2001). Beginning Literacy with Language. Baltimore, MD: Brooks.
[8] Doyle, A., Connolly, J., & Rivest, L. (1980). The Effect of Playmate Familiarity on the Social Interactions of Young Children. Child Development, 51, 217-223.
[9] Erting, C. J. (2003). Language and Literacy Development in Deaf Children: Implications of a Sociocultural Perspective. In: B. Bodner-Johnson, & M. Sass-Lehrer (Eds.), The Young Deaf or Hard of Hearing Child: A Family-Centered Approach to Early Education (pp. 373-398). Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.
[10] Esposito, B. G., & Koorland, M. A. (1989). Play Behavior of Hearing Impaired Children: Integrated and Segregated Settings. Journal of Exceptional Children, 55, 412-419.
[11] Gatty, J. (1990). The Effects of Deafness on Play in Four-Year-Old Boys. Unpublished Dissertation, Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts.
[12] Gerner de Garcia, B. (2003). Acquisition of English Literacy by Signing Deaf Children. Ponto de Vista: Revista de Educado e Processos Inclusivos, No. 5, 129-150.
[13] Gillen, J., & Hall, N. (2001). “Hiya, Mum!”: An Analysis of Pretence Telephone Play in a Nursery Setting. Early Years, 21, 15-24.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575140123969
[14] Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Van Lieshout, R., & Duff, D. (2000). Directiveness in Teachers’ Language Input to Toddlers and Preschoolers in Day Care. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1101-1114.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4305.1101
[15] Gregory, S., & Mogford, K. (1983). The Development of Symbolic Playing Young Deaf Children. In D. Rogers, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), The Acquisition of Symbolic Skills (pp. 221-231). New York: Springer.
[16] Halliday, M. A. K. (1974). Explorations in the Functions of Language. London: Edward Arnold.
[17] Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning How to Mean: Explorations in the Development of Language. London: Edward Arnold.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-443701-2.50025-1
[18] Halliday, M. A. K. (1978). A Social-Functional Approach to Language. In M.A.K. Halliday (Ed.), Language as Social Semiotic (pp. 36-58). London: Edward Arnold.
[19] Halliday, M. A. K. (1979). One Child’s Protolanguage. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication (pp. 171-190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[20] Higginbotham, D., & Baker, B. (1981). Social Participation and Cognitive Play Differences in Hearing Impaired and Normally Hearing Preschoolers. The Volta Review, 83, 135-149.
[21] Hoff, E. (2003). The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development via Maternal Speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1378.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612
[22] Hoff, E. (2010). Context Effects on Young Children’s Language Use: The Influence of Conversational Setting and Partner. First Language, 30, 461-472.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723710370525
[23] Isbell, R. T., & Raines, S. (1991). Young Children’s Oral Language Productions in Three Types of Play Centers. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 5, 140-146.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02568549109594811
[24] Johnson, R., Liddell, S., & Erting, C. (1989). Unlocking the Curriculum: Principles for Achieving Access in Deaf Education. Washington DC: Gallaudet University.
[25] Krestchmer, R. (1972). A Study to Assess the Play Activities and Gesture Output of Hearing Handicapped Preschool Children. Project No. 45-2109, Cincinnati Speech and Hearing Center: US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Office of Education, Bureau of the Handicapped.
[26] Kuntze, M. (1998). Literacy and Deaf Children: The Language Question. Topics in Language Disorders, 18, 1-15.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00011363-199808000-00003
[27] Lederberg, A. R., & Spencer, P. E. (2001). Vocabulary Development of Young Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children. In M. D. Clark, M. Marschark, & M. Karchmer (Eds.), Context, Cognition, and Deafness (pp. 73-92). Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press.
[28] Mahshie, S. N. (1997). A First Language: Whose Choice Is It? Washington DC: Gallaudet University Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center.
http://clerccenter.gallaudet.edu/documents/clerc/SI-AFirstLanguage.pdf
[29] Mann, L. F. (1984). Play Behaviors of Deaf and Hearing Children. In D. S. Martin (Ed.), International Symposium on Cognition, Education, and Deafness (pp. 27-32). Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press.
[30] Marshall, H., & McCandless, B. (1957). A Study of Prediction of Social Behavior of Preschool Children. Child Development, 28, 149-159.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1125877
[31] Mayberry, R. I. (2002). Cognitive Development of Deaf Children: The Interface of Language and Perception in Neuropsychology. In S. J. Segaolwitz, & I. Rapin (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (2nd ed., Vol. 8, Part II, pp. 71-107). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
[32] Meadow, K., Greenberg, M., Erting, C., & Carmichael, H. (1981). Interactions of Deaf Mothers and Deaf Preschool Children: Comparisons with Three Other Groups of Deaf and Hearing Dads. American Annals of the Deaf, 126, 454-468.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.1463
[33] Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative Data Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
[34] Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). When Parents Are Deaf vs. Hard of Hearing: Patterns of Sign Use and School Placement of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education, 9, 133-152.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enh017
[35] Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2005). Parental Hearing Status and Signing among Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students. Sign Language Studies, 5, 83-96.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sls.2005.0004
[36] Moores, D. (2001). Educating the Deaf: Psychology, Principles, and Practices (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.
[37] Morford, J. P., & Mayberry, R. I. (2000). A Reexamination of “Early Exposure” and Its Implications for Language Acquisition by Eye. In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. I. Mayberry (Eds.), Language Acquisition by Eye (pp. 111-128). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[38] Musyoka, M. (2015). Understanding Indoor Play in Deaf Children: An Analysis of Play Behaviors. Psychology, 6, 10-19.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2015.61002
[39] Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The Acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin, (Ed.), The Cross-Linguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Vol. 1, pp. 881-938). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[40] Olswang, L., Stoel-Gammon, C., Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1987). Assessing Prelinguistic and Early Linguistic Behaviors in Developmentally Young Children. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.
[41] Pershey, M. G. (1994). Children’s Awareness of Pragmatic Language Functions in Narrative Text. Dissertation Abstracts International, DA9434288.
[42] Pershey, M. G. (1997). Teaching Pragmatic Language Awareness as an Integral Aspect of Reading and Language Arts Instruction. Reading Horizons, 37, 299-314.
[43] Roskos, K., & Christie, J. (Eds.) (2000). Play and Literacy in Early Childhood: Research from Multiple Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[44] Schick, B., de Villiers, P. A., de Villiers, J. G., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: A Study of Deaf Children. Child Development, 78, 376-396.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x
[45] Schick, B., de Villiers, P., & de Villiers, J. (2002). Theory of Mind: Language and Cognition in Deaf Children. Rockville, MD: ASHA Leader.
[46] Schirmer, B. (1989). Relationship between Imaginative Play and Language Development in Hearing-Impaired Children. American Annals of the Deaf, 134, 219-222.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.0609
[47] Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The Development of Communication Skills: Modifications in the Speech of Young Children as a Function of Listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38, 1-38.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1165783
[48] Sontag, J. C. (1997). Contextual Factors Influencing the Sociability of Preschool Children with Disabilities in Integrated and Segregated Classrooms. Exceptional Children, 63, 389-404.
[49] Spencer, P. E. (1993). A Descriptive Study of Play by Deaf and Hearing Infants. Washington DC: Gallaudet Research Institute.
http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/custom/portlets/recordDetails/detailMini.jsp?_nfpb=true&_&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=ED385109&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&accno=ED385109
[50] Spencer, P. E. (1996). The Association between Language and Symbolic Play at Two Years: Evidence from Deaf Toddlers Child Development, 67, 867-881.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131866
[51] Spencer, P. E. (2000). Looking without Listening: Is Audition a Prerequisite for Normal Development of Visual Attention during Infancy? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 291-302.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/deafed/5.4.291
[52] Wiens, A. N., Molde, D. A., Holman, D. C., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1966). Can Interview Interaction Measures Be Taken from Tape Recordings? Journal f Psychology, 63, 249-260.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1966.10543038
[53] Woolfe, T., Herman, R., Roy, P., & Woll, B. (2010). Early Vocabulary Development in Native Deaf Signers: A BSL Adaptation of the Communicative Development Inventories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 322-331.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02151.x
[54] Yoshinaga-Itano, C., & Stredler-Brown, A. (1992). Learning to Communicate: Babies with Hearing Impairments Make Their Needs Known. Volta Review, 94, 107-129.

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.