Comparing the Latent Structure of the Children’s Category Test-Level 1 among Young Children and Older Adults: A Preliminary Study

Abstract

The aim of this paper was the comparison of the higher order nonverbal abilities, between young children and older adults. In specific, 42 kindergarten and 56 elementary school students (age range: 5 - 8 years), as well as 118 new-old adults and 27 old-old adults (age range: 61 - 88 years), were examined in the Children’s Category Test-Level 1 (CCT-1). Findings from the Confirmatory Factor Analyses that were applied to data, possibly reflect the delay of development of general abstraction and working memory in the group of kindergarten students and the decline of them in the group of old-old adults.

Share and Cite:

Papantoniou, G. , Moraitou, D. , Dinou, M. , Katsadima, E. , Savvidou, E. and Foutsitzi, E. (2016) Comparing the Latent Structure of the Children’s Category Test-Level 1 among Young Children and Older Adults: A Preliminary Study. Psychology, 7, 1352-1368. doi: 10.4236/psych.2016.711137.

1. Introduction

Researchers in the field of cognitive aging seem to agree that, on average, cognitive functioning declines with aging. Among the cognitive abilities that seem to be more affected by age is fluid intelligence (Baltes, 1997; Moraitou & Efklides, 2012; Salthouse, Pink, & Tucher-Drob, 2008) . Inference, inductive or deductive or heuristic, reflects processes that take an active part during problem solving in novel situations, independent of acquired knowledge. These processes underlie fluid intelligence (Demetriou, Spanoudis, Shayer, van der Ven, Brydges, Kroesbergen, Podjarny, & Swanson, 2014) . More specifically, according to the Dual-Process Model of Life Span Intellectual Development “the fluid cognitive mechanics are indexed by the speed and accuracy of elementary processes of information processing: information input; visual and motor memory; and basic perceptual-cognitive processes such as discrimination, comparison, categorization, as well as their application in working memory” (Baltes, 1997) . Furthermore, regarding the fluid mechanics, life span theory predicts a form of an inverted U-shape function across the life span, with decline beginning in young adulthood (see Baltes, 1997; Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Horn & Hofer, 1992 ), while the information processing theories propose that the developmental differences in fluid intelligence reflect differences in processing speed or working memory (see Demetriou et al., 2014 ).

Moreover, researchers have observed the general relationship between aging and development as findings in gerontological research have indicated that the collapse of intelligence in dementia patients causes retrogression to childhood and/or appears to reverse Piaget’s developmental stages (de Ajuriaguerra & Tissot, 1968; Rubial-Álvarez, de Sola, Machado, Sintas, Böhm, Sánchez-Benavides, Langohr, Muñiz, & Peña-Casanova, 2013; Shoji, Fukushima, Wakayana, Shizuka-Ikeda, Ikeda, Kawakami, Sakazume, Ikeda, Harigaya, Matsubara Kawarabayashi Murakami, Nagano, Manabe, & Abe, 2002) . As stated by the retrogenic models, there is an inverse and progressive pattern of functional and cognitive decline observed in Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) patients compared to the developmental acquisition of the corresponding capacities in children. Retrogenesis has been defined as the process by which degenerative mechanisms reverse the order of acquisition in normal development (Reinsberg, Franssen, Hasan, Monteiro, Boksay, Souren, Kenowsky, Auer, Elahi, & Kluger, 1999a; Reinsberg, Franssen, Souren, Auer, Akram, & Kenowsky, 2002; Reinsberg, Kenowsky, Franssen, Auer, & Souren, 1999b) .

The findings regarding the retrogenic models suggest that comparisons should be made between the cognitive ability of these two groups of population, namely the developing children and the retrograding elderly people. This suggestion has been taken into account in the limited use, in assessing cognition in moderate and severe AD patients, of several screening instruments available for cognitive examination in infancy and childhood. For example, the application of the adapted and modified Ordinal Scales of Psychological Developmental (OSPD), developed by Uzgiris and Hunt for cognitive testing in infants, has been found to be superior to the use of traditional tests, such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), in evaluating the cognitive ability of patients with moderately severe and severe AD (Borza, 2012) . It was also reported that the use of test measures previously applied to infants and young children, based on Piagetian tradition, demonstrated residual cognitive capacity in subjects with severe AD previously considered untestable (Da Silva, Bueno, & Bertolucci, 2011; Reinsberg et al., 1999a) . Furthermore, childhood intelligence test measures, such as the Japanese version of the Binet scale, namely the Tanaka-Binet intelligence scale, has been found to be useful in evaluating cognition in moderate to severe AD patients (Borza, 2012) .

Since there is a loss of cognitive abilities acquired during childhood before the appearance of clinically detectable dementia (Shoji et al., 2002) , comparisons of cognitive ability should also be made between the aforementioned groups of population, namely the developing children and the cognitively normal older adults or preclinical AD patients.

As regards the preclinical AD patients with neurological changes, it was reported that they do not demonstrate measurable cognitive decline on standard tests (Goldman, Price, Storandt, Grant, McKeel, Rubin, & Morris, 2001; Schmitt, Davis, Wekstein, Smith, Ashford, & Markesbery, 2000) . More sensitive tests to detect early or preclinical stages of AD are desired, such as the MMSE, Wechler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) and the Wechler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R), which are evidence- based test batteries for memory decline and intelligence. However, these scales can be administered (have been developed and standardized) only to persons between 16 and 74 years of age (Shoji et al., 2002) .

In order for an approximate comparison between children’s and older adults’ cognitive ability to be correctly conducted, the administration of the same screening instruments to the two groups should be available (Papantoniou, Moraitou, Dinou, Katsadima, Savvidou, & Foutsitzi, 2015; Rubial-Alvarez, Machado, Sintas, de Sola, Böhm, & Peña-Casanova, 2007; Shoji et al., 2002) . In addition, throughout the many attempts that have been made to develop test batteries for the early detection of dementia, it is also possible that the detection of preclinical AD could be more accurate through neuropsychological tests focused on the detection of developmental disturbances both in young children and older adults. Therefore, simple screening instruments, available for cognitive examination in infancy and childhood, are needed to allow the assessment of the cognitive ability in older adults, in a short period of time, which would assist in early detection of cognitive impairment.

1.1. Children’s Category Test

Although, there is an increase in the number of the neuropsychological tests designed to evaluate brain dysfunction in children, until recently there were few measures of novel problem-solving for children that were adequately standardized and normed. The Children’s Category Test (CCT; Boll, 1993 ) is included among them due to its age- based norms that were based on a large standardization sample that was representative in the U.S. population.

The CCT is an instrument originally created from short forms of the child versions of the Halstead Category Test (HCT; Halstead, 1947 ) in an effort to reduce lengthy administration times of the original versions, providing a more efficient and user-friendly version of the HCT, which has established sensitivity to acquired cerebral impairment (Allen, Caron, & Goldstein, 2006) . The CCT is an instrument designed to assess non- verbal learning and memory, concept formation and problem-solving abilities with novel material. It provides information on the child’s ability to develop alternative solutions, to change problem-solving strategies, to benefit from experience, and to develop, test, and modify hypotheses. This constellation of cognitive processes is highly related to fluid intelligence. The CCT may be used to determine whether or not a child is able to perform the aforementioned processes despite the existence of learning disorders, verbal or motor deficits, neurological deficits, or emotional handicaps (Boll, 1993) . The Children’s Category Test consists of two levels. Level one (CCT-1) was developed for children of 5 - 8 years of age, and Level two (CCT-2) was intended for ages 9 - 16. Both levels require from the child to create and modify strategies of responding to visual stimuli based on corrective feedback.

Taking into account the aforementioned findings, Allen, Knatz, & Mayfield (2006) used Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in order to examine the construct validity of the CCT-1, in a clinical sample with heterogeneous forms of brain dysfunction, by contrasting the factor structure reported in the standardization sample (Donders, 1999) , to the one indirectly suggested by Moore et al. (2004) . The factor structure found in Allen et al.’s (2006) clinical sample, using CFA, was similar to the one found in the standardization sample (Donders, 1999) , with subtests II and III forming one factor, and subtests IV and V forming another. Allen et al.’s (2006) results support viewing CCT-1 as a multidimensional procedure that assesses two distinct but related constructs (factors). The first construct, “Conceptual Discrimination”, is consistent with the view that the CCT-1 is a measure of problem solving and reasoning. The second, “Conceptual Memory”, appears to assess learning and memory, and as such departs from what might be traditionally thought of as a test of reasoning ability (Allen et al., 2006) .

1.2. Aim of the Study

Comparing pair-wise the four groups of our sample (first- to second-grade elementary school students with new-old adults, & kindergarten students with old-old adults), the latent structure of the CCT-1 was expected to differentiate between first- to second-grade elementary school students and new-old adults, on the one hand and kindergarten students and old-old adults, on the other (Hypothesis 1).

In specific, we expected to find similar latent structure of the CCT-1 for first- to second-grade elementary school students and new-old adults (Hypothesis 2a), and similar latent structure of the CCT-1 for kindergarten students and old-old adults (Hypothesis 2b).

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The total sample consisted of four groups of individuals: a group of kindergarten students, a group of first- to second-grade elementary school students, a group of new-old adults, and a group of old-old adults.

The first group comprised 42 kindergarten students 5 to 6 years old (mean age = 68.1 months, age range: 61 - 75 months). Of the 42 participants, 16 were boys (38.1%) and 26 were girls (61.9%). The second group included 56 first- to second-grade elementary school students 6 to 8 years old (mean age = 85.45 months, age range: 74 - 98 months). Of the 56 participants, 22 were boys (39.3%) and 34 were girls (60.7%). None of the children had a history of learning difficulties (based on the school records and student reports) and they all attended the regular curriculum in two preschool institutions (one public and one private) and three primary schools (two public and one private) of medium and high socioeconomic status, in the city of Ioannina in Epirus (a province in the West of Greece).

Furthermore, two groups of older adults were tested. In specific, one group comprised 118 new-old adults (mean age = 71.33 years, age range: 61 - 79 years). Of the 118 participants, 45 were men (38.1%) and 73 were women (61.9%). The second group of older adults included 27 old-old adults (mean age = 83.04 years, age range: 80 - 88 years). Of the 27 participants, 10 were men (37.0%) and 17 were women (63.0%). Exclusion criteria for both groups were a history of neurological conditions or psychiatric diseases, alcohol or drug abuse, severe head trauma, profound visual impairments, and verbal incomprehension. None of the participants had been diagnosed as meeting the diagnostic criteria for possible dementia, by consultant neurologists and psychiatrists. All the participants were community dwelling adults-volunteers recruited by the researchers from seniors’ day-care centers. They were residents of Thessaloniki and Kozani (a town in the province of West Macedonia in Greece). It should be noted that the subsample of older adults included an over representation (57.2%) of persons with 9 years of formal education or fewer.

2.2. Instrument

Children’s Category Test-Level 1 (CCT-1): The CCT-1 (Boll, 1993) is an individually administered test designed for children between the ages of 5 to 8 years. It is presented in booklet form and consists of 80 items distributed across 5 subtests. Each of the subtests I and V comprises 10 items while each of the subtests II, III and IV comprises 20 items.

The items comprising each subtest are connected by a single principle which varies across the subtests. The test requires the child to identify the conceptual rule that underlies each subtest and apply that concept to answering each item correctly. The child is shown a series of pictures that is intended to suggest a particular color and it has to respond by pointing to (or verbally identifying) one of four colors printed in a response card. The child receives immediate feedback as to the correctness of each response and it is expected to use this feedback to determine the conceptual rule underlying the subtest. Subtest I can be employed as a practice test to determine if the child understands the task and is able to provide appropriate responses, while subtest V requires the child to remember and apply the principles of the previous subtests. The conceptual rules underlying the subtests include the following: color recognition (subtest I), determination of the relative quantity of a specific color (subtest II), identification of the oddity in shape or size (subtest III), identification of the missing color (subtest IV), and review of the principles presented in subtests I through IV (subtest V).

As regards CCT-1 reliability, averaged coefficient, based on Fisher’s z transformation, is 0.88 demonstrating that CCT-1 possesses a high degree of internal consistency. The averaged test-retest reliability for the total error T score is 0.79 indicating that scores on the CCT-1 possess adequate stability across short time of periods. In specific, higher reliability coefficients are expected as the time between testing increases to 2 to 6 months (Boll, 1993) .

2.3. Procedure

As regards the participation of the kindergarten and elementary school students in the study, in collaboration with the school committees, their parents were asked to give a written statement of consent prior to the testing and then they completed an individual-demographics form. Children’s testing in CCT-1 was performed at an individual basis, in their school environment. No time limit was assigned for the completion of the tests and all young participants were informed that they were free to withdraw from testing at any time.

Older adults were also examined at an individual basis, either at the center from which they had been recruited, or in their own home. No time limit was assigned for the completion of the examination and the participants were informed that they were free to withdraw from testing at any time. All the participants signed an informed consent and then they completed an individual-demographics form.

The test was administered according to standard procedures by a psychologist and doctoral practicum students who were trained to the reliable and valid administration of the test.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

According to the introduction section, previous empirical works have implied two alternative two-factor models for the CCT-1 dimensionality. The aforementioned two- factor models were a priori competing with each other, as well as with a one-factor model in which subtests II, III, IV, and V were specified to load on a single factor (Allen et al., 2006) . Hence, taking into account findings supporting that factor structure of neurocognitive measures may vary from one population to the next (Woodward & Donders, 1998) , we used CFA in order to investigate possible qualitative changes in the latent structure of the CCT-1, between developing children and cognitively healthy older adults.

Since the CCT-1 manual does not provide normative data per subtest and because of the relatively small sample size of each group, the total raw numbers of errors per subtest on CCT-1 were used in the analysis of the present study. Subtest I was not included in the CFA because only 20 (8.2%) participants (4 kindergarten students, 6 elementary school students, and 10 new-old adults) made errors on subtest I and the rest (including all of the old-old adults) had completely flawless performance. As a result the distribution of error scores for subtest I did not meet assumptions of multivariate normality, which is desirable for factor analyses (see Allen et al., 2006; Moore et al., 2004 ). Therefore, each covariance matrix was based on four total scores (measured variables), namely, total raw score for subtest II, total raw score for subset III, total raw score for subset IV, and total raw score for subset V.

CFA was conducted in EQS Version 6.1 (Bentler, 2005) and performed on the five covariance matrices, which stemmed from the total sample and each one of the four groups of participants, using the Maximum Likelihood (ML or ML ROBUST) estimation method. The Wald test was used to test the need for the estimated parameters and to suggest a more restricted model.

3. Results

3.1. Testing Latent Structure of CCT-1 in the Total Sample

Initially, we compared the three following factor structures, in the total sample (Models A): Model A1 [the one-factor model in which all four measured variables loaded on a single latent variable], Model A2 [the two-factor model which proposed a factor consisting of subtests II and IV, and a second factor consisted of subtests III and V, with interrelations between the two factors] and Model A3 [the two-factor model which proposed a factor consisting of subtests II and III, and a second factor consisted of subtests IV and V, with interrelations between the two factors]. Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for the CFA Models A are presented in Table 1.

As shown in Table 1, the two-factor Model A3 provided the best fit of the data when compared to the Models A1 and A2. For Model A3, the χ2 was not significant and the other Goodness-of-Fit Indexes were excellent (Brown, 2006) . According to the suggestions of the Wald test all the parameters’ loadings of Model A3 were statistically significant. The Model A3 is displayed in Figure 1.

Table 1. Summary of fit tests for confirmatory factor analysis models testing latent structure of CCT-1 in the total sample.

Figure 1. The two-factor latent structure of the CCT-1 in the total sample and the subsamples of kindergarten students, elementary school students and new-old adults (standardized solution): Models A3, B3, C3, & D3. (*All loadings drawn indicate significant associations (p < 0.05). **e = measurement error).

3.2. Testing Latent Structure of CCT-1 in the Subsamples of Elementary School Students and New-Old Adults

In order to compare the latent structure in CCT-1 between elementary school students and new-old adults, we tested the three aforementioned factor structures, in the group of first- to second-grade elementary school students (Models B). Due to the statistically significant excess kurtosis of this group of the sample, Models B1, B2 and B3 were computed using the Maximum Likelihood (ML ROBUST) estimation method. Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for the CFA Models B are presented in Table 2.

As can be seen in Table 2, according to their indexes, it seemed that Models B1, B2, and B3 should be considered as equivalent. Since the Δχ2 among the three models was not statistically significant, they seemed to fit the data equally well. However, according to the suggestions of the Wald test, the Model B1 was not fully confirmed, for the group of elementary school students, because the variance of the single latent variable (factor) was not found to be statistically significant (p = 0.25). Similarly, the Model B2 was not fully confirmed, for the group of elementary school students, because the variance of the factor, consisting of subtests II and IV, was not found to be statistically significant either (p = 0.32).

(a) (b)

Table 2. (a) Summary of fit tests for confirmatory factor analysis models testing latent structure of CCT-1 in the subsample of elementary school students. (b) Summary of fit tests for confirmatory factor analysis models testing latent structure of CCT-1 in the subsample of new-old adults.

*Given that additional support for the fit of the solution would be evidenced by a 90% confidence interval (CI) of the RMSEA whose upper limit is below the cutoff values of 0.08 - 0.10 (see Brown, 2006 ), at this point it should be mentioned that the width of the CI for RMSEA is affected by sample size (e.g., when N < 100) and the number of freely estimated parameters. Thus, in such cases the values and the width of the 90% CI for RMSEA should be treated cautiously and with less concern if all other indices are strongly in a range supporting “good” model fit ( Bollen, 1990; Brown, 2006 ).

Therefore, the two-factor Model B3 provided the best fit of the data when compared to Models B1 and B2. For Model B3, the Goodness-of-Fit Indexes were excellent (Brown, 2006) . According to the suggestions of the Wald test all the parameters’ loadings of Model B3 were statistically significant except for the measurement error of the indicator that represented the raw total error score of subtest V (p = 0.93). The Model B3 is displayed in Figure 1.

Then, we tested the three aforementioned factor structures, in the group of new-old adults (Models C). Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for the CFA Models C are presented in Table 2.

As shown in Table 2, the two-factor Model C3 provided the best fit of the data when compared to Models C1 and C2. For Model C3, the χ2 was not significant and the other Goodness-of-Fit Indexes were excellent (Brown, 2006) . According to the suggestions of the Wald test all the parameters’ loadings of Model C3 were statistically significant except for the measurement error of the indicator that represented the raw total error score of subtest V (p = 0.05). The Model C3 is displayed in Figure 1.

The comparison of the CFA Model B3 with the Model C3 indicates a similar latent structure in CCT-1 for first- to second-grade elementary school students and new-old adults and verified Hypothesis 2a.

3.3. Testing Latent Structure of CCT-1 in the Subsamples of Kindergarten Students and Old-Old Adults

In order to compare the latent structure in CCT-1 between kindergarten students and old-old adults, we tested the three aforementioned factor structures, in the group of kindergarten students (Models D). Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for the CFA Models D are presented in Table 3.

As can be seen in Table 3, for all the Models (D1, D2 & D3) the χ2 was not significant and the other Goodness-of-Fit Indexes were excellent (Brown, 2006) . Furthermore, since the Δχ2 among the three models was not statistically significant, they seemed to fit the data equally well. Therefore, according to their indexes, it seemed that Models D1, D2, and D3 should be considered as equivalent. Thus, we did not derive only a two- factor Model D3 that could explain the variance of the kindergarten students’ performance on CCT-1. We also derived two more Models (D1 & D2) that could explain the variance of the kindergarten students’ performance on CCT-1, equally well compared to the aforementioned Model D3.

It should be noted here, that according to the suggestions of the Wald test all the parameters’ loadings of Models D1, D2, and D3 were statistically significant except for the measurement error of the indicator that represented the raw total error score of subtest V, both for the Model D2 (p = 0.33) and Model D3 (p = 0.80). For the group of kindergarten students, Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0.65 for a scale consisted of all four measured variables loaded on the single latent variable. The Model D3 is displayed in Figure 1. The Model D2 is displayed in Figure 2, and the Model D1 is displayed in Figure 3.

Finally, we tested the three aforementioned factor structures, in the group of old-old

(a) (b)

Table 3. (a) Summary of fit tests for confirmatory factor analysis models testing latent structure of CCT-1 in the subsample of kindergarten students. (b) Summary of fit tests for confirmatory factor analysis models testing latent structure of CCT-1 in the subsample of old-old adults.

*Given that additional support for the fit of the solution would be evidenced by a 90% confidence interval (CI) of the RMSEA whose upper limit is below the cutoff values of 0.08 - 0.10 (see Brown, 2006 ), at this point it should be mentioned that the width of the CI for RMSEA is affected by sample size (e.g., when N < 100) and the number of freely estimated parameters. Thus, in such cases the values and the width of the 90% CI for RMSEA should be treated cautiously and with less concern if all other indices are strongly in a range supporting “good” model fit (Bollen, 1990; Brown, 2006) .

adults (Models E). Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for the CFA Models E are presented in Table 3.

As can be seen in Table 3, since the Δχ2 among the three models was not statistically significant, they seemed to fit the data equally well. However, according to the suggestions of the Wald test, the Model E2 was not fully confirmed, for the group of old-old adults, because the variance of the factor, consisting of subtests II and IV, was not found to be statistically significant (p = 0.14). Similarly, the Model E3 was not fully confirmed either, for the group of old-old adults, because the Root Mean-Square Error of Approximation index was not found to be acceptable (RMSEA = 0.13) (Brown, 2006) .

Therefore, the one-factor Model E1 in which all four measured variables loaded on a single latent variable provided the best fit of the data when compared to Models E2 and E3. For Model E1, the Goodness-of-Fit Indexes were excellent (Brown, 2006) . According to the suggestions of the Wald test all the parameters’ loadings of Model E1 were statistically significant. For the group of old-old adults, Cronbach’s α coefficient was .75 for a scale consisted of all four measured variables loaded on the single latent variable. The Model E1 is displayed in Figure 3.

The comparison of the CFA Models D1, D2, and D3, on the one hand, with the Model E1, on the other, does not indicate a quite similar pattern of structure in CCT-1 for kindergarten students and old-old adults and does not fully verify Hypothesis 2b.

Figure 2. The two-factor latent structure of the CCT-1 in the subsample of kindergarten students (standardized solution): Model D2. (*All loadings drawn indicate significant associations (p < 0.05). **e = measurement error).

To summarize, the aforementioned findings confirm Hypothesis 1 and support the existence of a different factor structure in CCT-1 between first- to second-grade elementary school students and new-old adults, on the one hand, and kindergarten students and old-old adults, on the other.

4. Discussion

The aim of the present study was the comparison of the higher order thinking abilities, which are related to fluid intelligence, between the developing children and the retrograding older adults through the investigation of the qualitative changes in the latent structure of the CCT-1, from age to age, using CFA. For the groups of elementary school students and new-old adults, the results of CFA seem to support the existence of two factors measured by the CCT-1, namely a “Conceptual Discrimination” factor, and a “Conceptual Memory” factor. These results are consistent with previous findings (see Allen et al., 2006; Donders, 1999 ) regarding the factor structure of the CCT-1.

In general terms, the different pattern in the latent factor structure of the CCT-1, that was found in the present study, between the groups of kindergarten students and old-old adults, on the one hand, and the groups of first- to second-grade elementary school students and new-old adults, on the other, supports the hypothesis of “retrogenesis”. However, our research findings must be interpreted with a few limitations in mind. The restricted nature of the sample should be noted with regard to the number of participants in each age group as well as to the comparative uniformity of 1) the educational status in the groups of the older adults and 2) the socioeconomic status in the groups of students. More research is also needed to further validate and refine, in other cultural contexts, the different pattern in the structure of the higher order thinking abilities, as they are measured by the CCT-1, that was found among the various groups of the present study.

5. Conclusion

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Allen, D. N., Caron, J. E., & Goldstein, G. (2006). Process Index Scores for the Halstead Category Test. In A. Poreh (Ed.), The Quantified Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment (pp. 259-279). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
[2] Allen, D. N., Knatz, D. T., & Mayfield. J. (2006). Validity of the Children’s Category Test-Level 1 in a Clinical Sample with Heterogeneous Forms of Brain Dysfunction. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 711-720.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.08.003
[3] Anderson, P. J., & Reidy, N. (2012). Assessing Executive Function in Preschoolers. Neuropsychology Review, 22, 345-360.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9220-3
[4] Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the Incomplete Architecture of Human Ontogeny. American Psychologist, 52, 366-380.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.366
[5] Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a Powerful Connection between Sensory and Cognitive Functions across the Adult Life Span: A New Window at the Study of Cognitive Aging? Psychology and Aging, 12, 12-21.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.12
[6] Bentler, P. M. (2005). EQS 6.1. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc.
[7] Boll, T. (1993). Children’s Category Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
[8] Bollen, K. A. (1990). Overall Fit in Covariance Structure Models: Two Types of Sample Size Effects. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 256.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.256
[9] Borza, L. R. (2012). The Concept of Retrogenesis: New Ways of Understanding Alzheimer’s Disease. Ph.D. Thesis, New York: New York University.
[10] Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York: Guilford.
[11] Carlson, S. M., Davis, A., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less Is More: Executive Function and Symbolic Representation in Preschool Children. Psychological Science, 16, 609-616.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01583.x
[12] Da Silva, R., Bueno, O., & Bertolucci, P. (2011). The Retrogenesis Theory to Classify Stages of Alzheimer’s Disease in a Sample of the Brazilian Population. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association, 7, 241-242.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.685
[13] de Ajuriaguerra, J., & Tissot, R. (1968). Some Aspects of Psychoneurologic Disintegration in Senile Dementia. In C. H. Mueller & L. Ciompi (Eds.), Senile Dementia (pp. 69-79). Switzerland: Huber.
[14] Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., Shayer, M., Mouyi, A., Kazi, S., & Platsidou, M. (2013). Cycles in Speed-Working Memory-G Relations: Towards a Developmental-Differential Theory of Mind. Intelligence, 41, 34-50.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.010
[15] Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., Shayer, M., van der Ven, S., Brydges, C. R., Kroesbergen, E., Podjarny, G., & Swanson, H. L. (2014). Relations between Speed, Working Memory, and Intelligence from Preschool to Adulthood: Structural Equation Modeling of 14 Studies. Intelligence, 46, 107-121.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.013
[16] Donders, J. (1999). Latent Structure of the Children’s Category Test at Two Age Levels in the Standardization Sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 279-282.
[17] Fajgelj, S., Bala, G., & Katic, R. (2010). Latent Structure of Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices. Collegium Antropologicum, 34, 1015-1026.
[18] Goldman, W. P., Price, J. L., Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., McKeel Jr., D. W., Rubin, E. H., & Morris, J. C. (2001). Absence of Cognitive Impairment or Decline in Preclinical Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 56, 361-367.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.56.3.361
[19] Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and Intelligence: A Quantitative Study of the Frontal Lobes. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
[20] Horn, J. L., & Hofer, S. M. (1992). Major Abilities and Development in the Adult Period. In R. J. Sternberg, & C. A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual Development (pp. 44-99). New York: Cambridge University Press.
[21] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
[22] Moore, B. A., Donders, J., & Thompson. E. H. (2004). Validity of the Children’s Category Test-Level 1 after Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 1-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/19.1.1
[23] Moraitou, D., & Efklides, A. (2012). The Wise Thinking and Acting Questionnaire: The Cognitive Facet of Wisdom and Its Relation with Memory, Affect, and Hope. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13, 849-873.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-011-9295-1
[24] Papantoniou, G., Moraitou, D., Dinou, M., Katsadima, E., Savvidou, E., Foutsitzi, E., & Masoura, E. (2015). Comparing the Latent Structure of Raven’s Educational Coloured Progressive Matrices among Young Children and Older Adults: A Preliminary Study. Hellenic Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 18, 122-130.
[25] Reinsberg, B., Franssen, E. H., Hasan, S. M., Monteiro, I., Boksay, I., Souren, L. E. M., Kenowsky, S., Auer, S. R., Elahi, S., & Kluger, A. (1999a). Retrogenesis: Clinical, Physiologic, and Pathologic Mechanisms in Brain Aging, Alzheimer’s and Other Dementing Processes. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, S28-S36.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/PL00014170
[26] Reinsberg, B., Franssen, E. H., Souren, L. E. M., Auer, S. R., Akram, Ι., & Kenowsky, S. (2002). Evidence and Mechanisms of Retrogenesis in Alzheimer’s and Other Dementias: Management and Treatment Import. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 17, 202-212.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/153331750201700411
[27] Reinsberg, B., Kenowsky, S., Franssen, E. H., Auer, S. R., & Souren, L. E. M. (1999b). Towards a Science of Alzheimer’s Disease Management: A Model Based upon Current Knowledge of Retrogenesis. International Psychogeriatrics, 11, 7-23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610299005554
[28] Rubial-álvarez, S., de Sola, S., Machado, M. C., Sintas, E., Bohm, P., Sánchez-Benavides, G., Langohr, K., Muniz, R., & Pena-Casanova, J. (2013). The Comparison of Cognitive and Functional Performance in Children and Alzheimer’s Disease Supports the Retrogenesis Model. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 33, 191-203.
[29] Rubial-Alvarez, S., Machado, M.-C., Sintas, E., de Sola, S., Bohm, P., & Pena-Casanova, J. (2007). A Preliminary Study of the Mini-Mental State Examination in a Spanish Child Population. Journal of Child Neurology, 22, 1269-1273.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0883073807307098
[30] Salthouse, T. A. (2005). Relations between Cognitive Abilities and Measures of Executive Functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 532-545.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.532
[31] Salthouse, T. A., Pink, J. E., & Tucher-Drob, E. M. (2008). Contextual Analysis of Fluid Intelligence. Intelligence, 36, 464-486.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.10.003
[32] Savvidou, E., Papantoniou, G., Moraitou, D., Dinou, M., Katsadima, E., Foutsitzi, E., & Tsentidou, G. (2016). Comparing the Latent Structure of the Mini-Mental State Examination among Young Children and Older Adults: A Preliminary Study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. Conference Abstract: SAN2016 Meeting.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/conf.fnhum.2016.220.00077
[33] Schmitt, F. A., Davis, D. G., Wekstein, D. R., Smith, C. D., Ashford, J. W., & Markesbery, W. R. (2000). “Preclinical” AD Revisited: Neuropathology of Cognitive Normal Older Adults. Neurology, 55, 370-376.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.3.370
[34] Shoji, M., Fukushima, K., Wakayana, M., Shizuka-Ikeda, M., Ikeda, Y., Kawakami, A., Sakazume, Y., Ikeda, M., Harigaya, Y., Matsubara, E., Kawarabayashi, T., Murakami, T., Nagano, I., Manabe, Y., & Abe, K. (2002). Intellectual Faculties in Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease Regress to the Level of a 4 - 5-Year-Old Child. Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 2, 143-147.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1444-1586.2002.00040.x
[35] Woodward, H., & Donders, J. (1998). The Performance of Children with Traumatic Head Injury on the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning-Screening. Applied Neuropsychology, 5, 113-119.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an0503_1
[36] Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and Adolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354-360.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.