Inhibition in Action–Inhibitory Components in the Behavioral Activation System
Stefan Sütterlin, Stein Andersson, Claus Vögele
.
DOI: 10.4236/jbbs.2011.13021   PDF    HTML     6,716 Downloads   13,006 Views   Citations

Abstract

Over the past two decades, the neurobiological substrates of the reinforcement theory have been discussed in terms of a behavioral activation system (BAS) and a behavioral inhibition system (BIS). While the BAS has been conceptualized as both an activating system and an approach-related system, the empirical evidence for either approach remains inconclusive. In the current study we hypothesize that the inclusion of self-regulatory capacity contributes to a better understanding of the BAS. In a sample of 29 volunteers motor response inhibition elicited by a stop-signal task and heart rate variability (HRV) as a proxy of self-regulatory capacity were related to BAS scores (BIS/BAS scales [1]). Results show significant positive associations between inhibitory capacity and the sensitivity of the behavioral activation system, suggesting markers of self-regulation as components of the BAS.

Share and Cite:

Sütterlin, S. , Andersson, S. and Vögele, C. (2011) Inhibition in Action–Inhibitory Components in the Behavioral Activation System. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 1, 160-166. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2011.13021.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] C. S. Carver and T. L. White, “Behavioral Inhibition, Behavioral Activation, and Affective Responses to Impending Reward and Punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 67, No. 2, 1994, pp. 319-333. HHdoi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319
[2] J. A. Gray, “Précis of the Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System,” The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 5, No. 3, 1982, pp. 469-534. HHdoi:10.1017/S0140525X00013066
[3] J. A. Gray and N. McNaughton, “The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: Reprise,” In: D. A. Hope, Ed., Perspectives on Anxiety, Panic and Fear, University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, 1996, pp. 61-134.
[4] J. J. Simon, S. W. Walther, C. J. Fiebach, H. C. Friederich, C. Stippich, M. Weisbrod and S. Kaiser, “Neural Processing is Modulated by Approach- and Avoidance-Related Personality Traits,” NeuroImage, Vol. 49, No. 2, 2010, pp. 1868-1874. HHdoi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.016
[5] M. R. M. Scholten, J. van Honk, A. Aleman and R. S. Kahn, “Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), Behavioral Activation System (BAS) and Schizophrenia: Relationship with Psychopathology and Physiology,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, Vol. 40, No. 7, 2006, pp. 638-645.
[6] S. K. Sutton and R. J. Davidson, “Prefrontal Brain Asymmetry: A Biological Substrate of the Behavioral Approach and Inhibition Systems,” Psychological Science, Vol. 8, No. 3, 1997, pp. 204-210. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x
[7] R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs, Eds., “Handbook of Self-Regulation,” The Guilford Press, New York, 2004.
[8] J. E. Richards, “Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Predicts Heart Rate and Visual Responses during Visual Attention in 14- and 20-Week-Old Infants,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 22, No. 1, 1985, pp. 101-109. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01567.x
[9] N. A. Fox, “Psychophysiological Correlates of Emotional Reactivity during the First Year of Life,” Developmental Psychology, Vol. 25, No. 3, 1989, pp. 364-372. HHdoi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.364
[10] S. L. Brenner, T. P. Beauchaine and P. D. Sylvers, “A Comparison of Psychophysiological and Self-Report Measures of BAS and BIS Activation,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 42, No. 1, 2005, pp. 108-115. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00261.x
[11] H. L. Movius and J. J. B. Allen, “Cardiac Vagal Tone, Defensiveness, and Motivational Style,” Biological Psychology, Vol. 68, No. 2, 2005, pp. 147-162. HHdoi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.019
[12] S. W. Porges, “Vagal Tone: An Autonomic Mediator of Affect,” In: J. Garber and K. A. Dodge, Eds., Principles of Psychophysiology: Physical, Social, and Inferential Elements, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1991, pp. 456-512.
[13] S. W. Porges, “Orienting in a Defensive World: Mammalian Modifications of Our Evolutionary Heritage. A Polyvagal Theory,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 32, No. 4, 1995, pp. 301-318. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01213.x
[14] N. Eisenberg, R. A. Fabes, B. Murphy, P. Maszk, M. Smith and M. Karbon, “The Role of Emotionality and Regulation in Children’s Social Functioning: A Longitudinal Study,” Child Development, Vol. 66, No. 5, 1995, pp. 1360-1384. HHdoi:10.2307/1131652
[15] E. E. Benarroch, “The Central Autonomic Network: Functional Organization, Dysfunction, and Perspective,” Mayo Clinic Proceedings, Vol. 68, No. 10, 1993, pp. 988-1001.
[16] J. F. Thayer and J. F. Brosschot, “Psychosomatics and Psychopathology: Looking up and down from the Brain,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 30, No. 10, 2005, pp. 1050-1058. HHdoi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.014
[17] J. F. Thayer and R. D. Lane, “Claude Bernard and the Heart-Brain Connection: Further Elaboration of a Model of Neurovisceral Integration,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 33, No. 2, 2009, pp. 81-88. HHdoi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004
[18] M. Boecker, M. M. Buecheler, M. L. Schroeter and S. Gauggel, “Prefrontal Brain Activation during Stop-Signal Response Inhibition: An Event-Related Functional Near- Infrared Spectroscopy Study,” Behavioural Brain Research, Vol. 176, No. 2, 2007, pp. 259-266. HHdoi:10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.009
[19] G. D. Logan and W. B. Cowan, “On the Ability to Inhibit thought and Action: A Theory of an Act of Control,” Psychological Review, Vol. 91, No. 3, 1984, pp. 295-327. HHdoi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.295
[20] G. D. Logan, “On the Ability to Inhibit Thought and Action: A User’S Guide to the Stop-Signal Paradigm,” In: D. Dagenbach and T. H. Carr, Eds., Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language, Academic Press, San Diego, 1994, pp. 184-239.
[21] G. G. Berntson, K. S. Quigley, J. F. Jang and S. T. Boysen, “An Approach to Artifact Identification: Application to Heart Period Data,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 27, No. 5, 1990, pp. 586-598. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb01982.x
[22] T. Kaufmann, S. Sütterlin, S. M. Schulz and C. V?gele, “ARTiiFACT: A Tool for Heart Rate Artifact Processing and Heart Rate Variability Analysis,” Behavior Research Methods, published online, 14 May 2011. HHdoi:10.3758/s13428-011-0107-7
[23] B. Allen, A. S. Chambers and D. N. Towers, “The Many Metrics of Cardiac Chronotropy: A Pragmatic Primer and a Brief Comparison of Metrics,” Biological Psychology, Vol. 74, No. 2, 2007, pp. 243-262. HHdoi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.08.005
[24] Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology, “Heart Rate Variability―Standards of Measurement, Physiological Interpretation, and Clinical Use,” European Heart Journal, Vol. 17, 1996, pp. 354-381.
[25] G. J. M. van Boxtel, M. W. van der Molen, J. R. Jennings and C. H. M. Brunia, “A Psychophysiological Analysis of Inhibitory Motor Control in the Stop-Signal ParaDigm,” Biological Psychology, Vol. 58, No. 3, 2001, pp. 229-262. HHdoi:10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00117-X
[26] S. W. Porges, J. A. Doussard-Roosevelt and A. K. Maiti, “Vagal Tone and the Physiological Regulation of Emotion,” In: Society for Research in Child Development, The Development of Emotion Regulation: Biological and Behavioral Considerations, Blackwell Publishing, New York, 1994, pp. 167-186.
[27] B. M. Appelhans and L. J. Luecken, “Heart Rate Variability as an Index of Regulated Emotional Responding,” Review of General Psychology, Vol. 10, No. 3, 2006, pp. 229-240. HHdoi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229
[28] V. Napadow, R. Dhond, G. Conti, N. Makris, E. N. Brown and R. Barbieri, “Brain Correlates of Autonomic Modulation: Combining Heart Rate Variability with fMRI,” NeuroImage, Vol. 42, No. 1, 2008, pp. 169-177. HHdoi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.238
[29] A. L. Hansen, B. H. Johnsen and J. F. Thayer, “Vagal Influence on Working Memory and Attention,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, Vol. 48, No. 3, 2003, pp. 263-274. HHdoi:10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00073-4
[30] S. Sütterlin, C. Herbert, M. Schmitt, A. Kübler and C. V?gele, “Frames, Decisions, and cardiac-Autonomic Control,” Social Neuroscience, Vol. 6, No. 2, 2011, pp. 169-177. HHdoi:10.1080/17470919.2010.495883
[31] S. Sütterlin, C. Herbert, M. Schmitt, A. Kübler and C. V?gele, “Overcoming Selfishness: Reciprocity, Inhibition, and Cardiac Autonomic Control in the Ultimatum Game,” Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 2, No. 173, 2011, pp. 1-8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.0017
[32] S. Funahashi, “Neuronal Mechanisms of Executive Control by the Prefrontal Cortex,” Neuroscience Research, Vol. 39, No. 2, 2001, pp. 147-165. HHdoi:10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00224-8
[33] E. Harmon-Jones and J. J. B. Allen, “Behavioral Activation Sensitivity and Resting Frontal EEG Asymmetry: Covariation of Putative Indicators Related to Risk of Mood Disorders,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 74, No. 5, 1997, pp. 1310-1316. HHdoi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1310
[34] J. Wacker, M. Heldmann and G. Stemmler, “Separating Emotion and Motivational Direction in Fear and Anger: Effects on Frontal Asymmetry,” Emotion, Vol. 3, No. 2, 2003, pp. 167-193. HHdoi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.2.167
[35] J. Hewig, D. Hagemann, J. Seifert, E. Naumann and D. Bartussek, “On the Selective Relation of Frontal Cortical Asymmetry and Anger-Out versus Anger-Control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 87, No. 6, 2004, pp. 926-939. HHdoi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.926
[36] J. Hewig, D. Hagemann, J. Seifert, E. Naumann and D. Bartussek, “The Relation of Cortical Activity and BIS/ BAS on the Trait Level,” Biological Psychology, Vol. 71, No. 5, 2006, pp. 42-53. HHdoi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.006
[37] A. F. Jorm, H. Christensen, A. S. Henderson, P. A. Jacomb, A. E. Korten and B. Rodgers, “Using the BIS/BAS Scales to Measure Behavioural Inhibition and Behavioural Activation: Factor Structure, Validity and Norms in a Large Community Sample,” Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 26, No. 1, 1998, pp. 49-58. HHdoi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00143-3
[38] A. J. Shackman, B. W. McMenamin, J. S. Maxwell, L. L. Greischar and R. J. Davidson, “Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Activity and Behavioral Inhibition,” Psychological Science, Vol. 20, No. 12, 2009, pp. 1500-1506. HHdoi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.