Interpersonal Climate Change Communication in Florida Using Quantile Regression

Abstract

Climate change is described as a potentially catastrophic phenomenon with the capacity to disrupt agricultural production, economies, health systems, education, and infrastructure, among other systems. In Florida, climate change is a concern because of the state’s extensive coastline and its influence on the economy, as well as residents’ safety and well-being. As early as 2007, researchers forecasted that vulnerable wetlands, mangroves, fisheries, and coastal infrastructure in Florida may be significantly damaged or destroyed by 2060. Climate change communication (CCC) is described as a complex problem that requires several layers of attention, especially in achieving the desired outcome of behavior change. Previous research suggested that climate change communicators would be more effective if they understood their audiences and their communication capacities. The purpose of the study was to determine the impact of demographic factors on social communication for residents of Florida. A survey was used to collect the data through an address-based sampling (ABS) method, where a total of 318 usable responses were received from Florida residence 18 years or older. A latent construct for describing social communication (Social Communication Index [SCI]) was created as the dependent variable and was tested against eight variables using a quantile regression approach. Using quantiles in 0.1 intervals, the results showed that knowledge, age, income, newspaper use, urbanicity, and race affected the SCI in one or more quantiles. Social media, sex, and religiosity were insignificant throughout all quantiles. While most of the results align with previous research, there is the need for further probing into social communication on climate change to ensure that audience segments are provided with climate change information through the channels they primarily use.

Share and Cite:

Mike, M. and Israel, G. (2022) Interpersonal Climate Change Communication in Florida Using Quantile Regression. American Journal of Climate Change, 11, 37-58. doi: 10.4236/ajcc.2022.112003.

1. Introduction

In 1992, the Rio Declaration created an impetus for the world to address climate change, as scientists described the phenomenon as potentially catastrophic (Bertolotti & Catellani, 2014; Colombo, 2017). Climate change has impacted global development by affecting infrastructure, agricultural production, poverty, health, education, and fueling conflict (Elia et al., 2015). Psychological distance, public uncertainty, the perception that there is less chance of direct impact, and the idea that timing of the impact is set in the distant future were a few of the reasons highlighted in the literature that pointed to miscommunication about climate change (Brügger, 2020; Jones et al., 2017; Loy & Spence, 2020; Morrison et al., 2018; Ouariachi et al., 2017; Pong, 2021; Romsdahl, 2020; Schuldt et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018).

Climate change communication (CCC) is described as a complex problem that requires several layers of attention, especially in achieving the desired outcome of behavior change (Johnson, 2012; Wibeck, 2014). While discussions on CCC started in the mid-1990’s (Chadwick, 2018), the CCC movement was suggested to be initiated by James Hansen in 2005 who provided urgency to the issue (Bowen, 2008; Robbins, 2020; Russill, 2008; Russill & Nyssa, 2009; Van der Hel et al., 2018). Since then, research on CCC has focused on media reports (Kunelius et al., 2017), social media (Anderson & Huntington, 2017; Boulianne et al., 2020; Lindsey et al., 2018), and public interaction (Pong, 2021; Tàbara et al., 2017) among others. CCC was described as a niche area of study, and even in 2018, almost 20 years after the tipping point, CCC is still described as a fairly new area of research (Chadwick, 2018), as well as a micro-field of research (Ballantyne, 2016). The Common difficulty with CCC, as proposed by researchers, is the issue of personal relevance (Devkota & Phuyal, 2018; Pong, 2021). Nevertheless, researchers and communication scientists were interested in audience segmentation research to propose effective communication strategies for CCC (Hine et al., 2014). Climate change communicators would potentially be more effective if they better understood their audiences, and their communication capacities (Chadwick, 2018).

In Florida, climate change is a concern because the state’s coastline is influential on the economy, residents’ safety, and population well-being. A significant portion of Florida’s economy is based on tourism (Atzori et al., 2018), fishing (Lorenzen et al., 2017; Paukert et al., 2016), and coastal infrastructure (McAlpine & Porter, 2018; Shen et al., 2016), which are suggested to be threatened by climate change. As early as 2007, researchers forecasted that vulnerable wetlands, mangroves, fisheries, and coastal infrastructure in Florida may be significantly damaged or destroyed by 2060 (Stanton & Ackerman, 2007). To compound the CCC issue, a news release by Earth Justice (an environmental group) in 2015 said that state workers are being prohibited from saying the phrases “climate change” and “global warming” during former Governor Scott’s administration (Korten, 2015). Additionally, the frequency and intensity of natural disasters fueled by climate change are suggested to increase in the future (Field, 2018; Middelbeek et al., 2014; Woodward & Samet, 2018). For example, the 2017 hurricane season had 17 named storms and 10 hurricanes, where six of those 10 hurricanes labelled as major hurricanes (Trenberth et al., 2018; Wint, 2019) while the 2020 season had 30 names storms, 14 hurricanes, and 7 major hurricanes. The average Atlantic hurricane activity includes 12 storms and six hurricanes (Trenberth et al., 2018).

The current study is unique from previous climate communications studies covered in this paper by way of the analyses used. Hine et al. (2014) evaluated approximately 20 CCC audience segmentation studies and outlined methodologies used therein. Hine et al. (2014) found that the researchers used various cluster analyses, latent profile analysis, and group screening, but not QR. Several other authors used qualitative approaches to identify different trends and approaches in CCC (Ballantyne, 2016; Chadwick, 2018; Ouariachi et al., 2017). Researchers that used QR for climate change analyses used variables that were akin to, but not directly parallel with the demographic variables presented in the current study because of their objectives. The intensity and frequency of disasters, as well as associated costs (NOAA, 2022), fueled by climate change are also increasing, while some researchers underscore CCC fatigue and psychological distance as a hindrance to communicating about climate change. Because Florida is vulnerable to climate change, and audience segmentation research is important for messaging, the researchers in the current study sought to determine the demographic attributes that are influencing social communication of climate change in Florida.

Purpose and Objectives

The purpose of the study was to determine the impact of demographic factors on social communication for residents of Florida. Specific objectives were as follows:

Objective 1 (O1): Determine the demographic attributes that impact social communication of climate change.

Objective 2 (O2): Determine the impact of demographic factors as different levels of social interaction.

2. Literature Review

In previous research on climate change communication, demographic factors around the issue have been relatively consistent, especially on factors like age, religion, knowledge, and income. In a study on how leaders communicate about climate change in urban cities, Boussalis et al. (2018) found that leaders in places with high vulnerabilities to climate change are more likely to communicate about it. Florida was described as a highly vulnerable state because of the increased intensity of weather-related disasters due to climate change, and its dependence on the coastline for socio-economic activity (Atzori et al., 2018; Lorenzen et al., 2017; McAlpine & Porter, 2018; Paukert et al., 2016). In a study on 1006 individuals, Detenber et al. (2016) looked at the Six Americas Scale to assess climate change communication in Singapore. The Six Americas ranged from alarmed to dismissive, with four other distinct segments in between (Maibach et al., 2011). In the study by Detenber et al. (2016), 95% of the respondents fell into three main categories: concerned, disengaged, and passive. Their research found that the average age of someone in the concerned category was 39.91 years and had the highest income. This concerned group relied on traditional mass media to slightly higher than average amount but has engaged in a fair amount of communication about the environment (Detenber et al., 2016). However, this group is also neutral in the idea that their friends and family are trustworthy sources of information on climate change.

In alignment with other literature, mass media was found to be the predominant information channel for climate change (Anderson, 2011; Barkemeyer et al., 2017; Bolsen & Shapiro, 2018; Forchtner et al., 2018; Nerlich et al., 2010; Pong, 2021; Russill & Nyssa, 2009). Additionally, newspaper analyses have been conducted to understand the framing of climate change messages since this medium was widely used (Anderson, 2011; Devkota & Phuyal, 2018; Duan et al., 2017). Anderson (2011) observed that newspapers have a major agenda setting effect because of their influence on the discourse of prominent topics. However, in the farming community, Devkota and Phuyal (2018) found that newspapers were not a significant variable for farmers’ awareness of climate change issues. Though national newspapers were found to be considered trustworthy sources, reliance on newspapers as a primary source for accurate information is most common among adults with graduate degrees, and those 65 and older (Chadwick, 2018). Loy and Spence (2020) found that text news on climate communication reduced the psychological distance when compared to video communication on climate change with text input. They concluded that their sample (n = 400) from the United Kingdom and Bangladesh also preferred local news to reduce psychological distance on climate change.

In the study by Detenber et al. (2016), members of the disengaged group are slightly older than the concerned group, with an average of 42.48 years. However, the researchers found that they were below the average education level, as well had a lower income than the concerned group (Detenber et al., 2016). This group was least likely to trust their social groups in communicating about climate change and believe that citizens are not responsible for reducing climate change (Detenber et al., 2016). The passive group was the oldest in the Detenber et al. (2016) study, was the least educated, and had the lowest incomes. However, traditional news was their most dependable source on climate change communication, and has done a significant amount of pro-environmental communication Detenber et al. (2016). In terms of social communication, this group are the most likely to believe their friends and family on issues of climate change.

Researchers have discussed audience segmentation based on age, income, education, and race, among other attributes in the market for CCC (Arbuckle et al., 2017; Badullovich et al., 2020; Detenber et al., 2016; Duan et al., 2021; Hine et al., 2017; Khadka et al., 2021; Pong, 2021; Thomas et al., 2019; Wonneberger et al., 2020).

Concerning age, Arbuckle et al. (2017), Morrison et al. (2018) in an Australian study, and Leiserowitz et al. (2021) found similar results as Detenber et al. (2016), where older individuals tend to be unconcerned or dismissive about climate change. However, Leiserowitz et al. (2021) also found that younger, more educated women tend to be in the alarmed category. Duan et al. (2021) found that age was significant in explaining an indifidual’s climate change behavior, as well as their climate change concern. In a study by Hine et al. (2017) age was used as a significant covariate in the social dimensions of psychological distance on climate change, as well as climate change concern. Expanding on the literature on gender, younger women being more emotive and involved in climate change issues was found by Stevenson et al. (2018), who stated that this aligned with previous research on gender. Morrison et al. (2018) also highlighted individuals with lower levels of education, and lower incomes tend to be less concerned or dismissive about climate change.

Ballantyne (2016) found that public understanding of climate change was tied to people’s knowledge on the topic. Previous research on knowledge and media suggests a connection between media representations and scientific representations of climate change. This also speaks to the fundamental misconception that making climate change information available increases knowledge on the issue (Ballantyne, 2016; Chadwick, 2018; Nerlich et al., 2010; Ouariachi et al., 2017; Sakellari, 2015). Devkota and Phuyal (2018) found that knowledge on climate change was significant on farmer’s awareness of the issue, but only in certain areas. Researchers also found that the perception of climate change risk was tied to knowledge (Aksit et al., 2018). In investigating exposure to cinematographic representations of climate change, Sakellari (2015) found that exposure to this medium of information only motivated behavior change in the short term, but failed to have a long-term impact.

Ecklund et al. (2017) found that association between religion and climate change views were almost entirely accounted for by political affiliation and ideology. Leiserowitz et al. (2021) also stated that religion plays an important role in politics. In this light, political ideology can be a proxy for religiosity and vice versa. Thomas et al. (2019) suggested socioeconomic status, race may play a role in interacting with climate change, as some individuals may not have the resources to focus on the issue. Regarding race, Leiserowitz et al. (2021) found that 57% of African Americans were in either concerned or alarmed categories, and 49% White Americans were in either the concerned or alarmed categories. However, Latinos were more expressive that humans caused global warming than non-Latinos, making them more susceptible to communicate about climate change (Leiserowitz et al., 2021).

3. Methodology

A survey was used to collect data for this research through an address-based sampling (ABS) method. A third-party vendor provided the addresses. The survey was initiated in November 2016 to collect data on Florida residents that were older than 18. Most of the items on the survey were previously used in survey research on climate change (Leiserowitz et al., 2010, 2015; Maibach et al., 2009), ensuring a measure of validity and reliability. The ABS was comprised of two replicates: the first replicate had 1500 addresses, and the second replicate had 500 addresses. The survey using the first replicate was executed by mailing an initial survey packet in November 2016, followed by a reminder postcard, then a second survey packet, and final survey packet ended the process in January 2017. The second replicate was executed using a “web push” approach (Dillman et al., 2014) where a web uniform resource locator (URL) and personal identification number (PIN) was mailed to potential respondents. This replicate was initiated in January 2017. Of the 2000 ABS invitations, 25 surveys were completed online, and 293 were completed by postal mail for a total of 318 usable responses 17.0% response rate (AAPOR, 2016).

A latent construct for describing social communication was created as the dependent variable for this study. The Social Communication Index (SCI) is calculated as an average of the amount an individual communicates about climate change based on social interaction data collected. Items from the survey included telling people about climate change in the previous six months (Variable 1 [V1]); the amount of information shared with friends and colleagues (Variable 2 [V2]); the frequency of discussions with friends on climate change (Variable 3 [V3]), and the likelihood to be asked about climate change compared to friends (Variable 4 [V4]). These questions used unipolar scales, with responses for V1 having a three-point scale ranging from “told no one” to “told a lot of people” ranging from one to three points respectively. The second item (amount of information shared) used a four-point scale ranging from “give no information” to “give a great deal of information” ranging from one to four points respectively. The third question (frequency of discussion with friends) used a five-point scale ranging from “very often to never” ranging from one to five points respectively. Lastly, the fourth question used (likelihood to be asked about climate change) also used a five-point scale, but the range moved from “extremely likely” to “not at all likely” ranging from one to five points respectively. Participant SCI scores were calculated as the simple average of the numeric values of the responses to the identified questions. Averaging Likert scale scores is a common practice in quantitative research (Garas et al., 2018; Layfield et al., 2020; Saudek et al., 2018; Wulandari & Inoue, 2018). Recent research shows averaging Likert scores from as little as four questions (Gadoth et al., 2021), and as much as 23 (Erni et al., 2021). Responses for two of these questions (frequency of discussion and likelihood to be asked about climate change) were reverse coded to ensure all the questions ranked social communication from the lowest value to the highest value.

The test for reliability indicated strong internal consistency for the SCI, with a median Cronbach’s Alpha value of 0.853 for the ten imputations (see Imputing Missing Data below). The percent of common variance accounted for by the SCI construct was an average of 70.69% for V1 across all imputations. The average communalities across all imputations showed that 80.3% of V1, 68.3% of V2, 77.1% of V3, and 57.0% of V4 were accounted for by the extracted latent construct. Average factor loadings across all imputations were 0.896, 0.826, 0.778 and 0.755 for V1, V2, V3, and V4 respectively.

Similar procedures were conducted for Religiosity and the Knowledge index. Religiosity was operationalized using six items including attendance at religious functions, which was adopted from AP-NORC (2015; see also Huber and Huber, 2012). The other five items asked about the frequency of praying, thinking about religious issues, reading religious materials, participating in religious activities, and supporting a religious organization financially. The items on praying and thinking about religious issues were drawn from Huber and Huber’s (2012) Centrality of Religion Scale, while the others were added to reflect religious practices in the US (Lisa Lundy, personal communication, December 7, 2020). These five items used a five-point scale ranging from “never” to “very often”. The six-point scale of religious service attendance ranged from “more than once a week” to “never” and was reverse coded to allow all responses in the religiosity variable to run in ascending order. Average Cronbach’s alpha was 0.903 for the Religiosity index. The knowledge index was created by calculating the average of the responses of 10 true/false knowledge items on climate change adapted from Leiserowitz, Smith, and Marlon’s (2010) study. Items included “global warming will cause some places to get wetter, while others will get drier”, “the earth is actually cooling, not warming” and “the Earth’s climate has changed naturally in the past, therefore humans are not the cause of global warming”. Average Cronbach’s alpha was 0.740, while the average variance loadings were 33.1% for V1, 13.55% for V2, and 12.52% for V3 across all imputations.

3.1. Data Analysis Procedures

3.1.1. Procedures for Imputing Missing Data

The item missing pattern was examined and characterized as arbitrary (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Two-stage multiple imputation (Berglund, 2010) was used to estimate missing values for interval and ordinal variables. American Communities Survey 5-year data at the ZCTA5 level was used as auxiliary data in the imputation procedure for the ABS data. The multiple imputation procedure was conducted with SAS Proc MI, version 9.4 to create 10 imputations.

3.1.2. Procedures for Weighting the ABS Data

Because coverage error and nonresponse error can be exacerbated by low response rates, the imputed data sets were weighted to reflect population parameters for Florida (Biemer & Christ, 2008). The SPSSINC Rake procedure (IBM SPSS Statistics 25 2017) was employed to calculate weights. Raking variables included Sex, Hispanic Ethnicity, Race, Age, and Educational Attainment. Values for the control totals were obtained from the American Communities Survey 5-year estimates for 2015.

3.1.3. Quantile Regression

Quantile regression (QR) was first proposed by Koenker and Bassett (1978) who based the concept on the L_1 method (minimization of weighted deviations) (Eide & Showalter, 2007; Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017; Hübler, 2017; Koenker & Hallock, 2001; Salman et al., 2019; Zietz et al., 2008). QR can be described as dividing data into observations based on defined interval criteria set by the researcher (Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017). For the 0.5 quantile (the median), symmetric weights are used, but for other quantiles, like the 0.1 quantile, asymmetric weights are applied (Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017; Zietz et al., 2008). One advantage of QR is that, unlike OLS, QR can be used to explain the determinants of the dependent variable at any point in the distribution (Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017; Hübler, 2017; Zietz et al., 2008). That is, instead of providing an averaged view of the data using OLS, one can evaluate relationships at a single point, and between different points of the data (Moutinho et al., 2017). Another advantage of QR over OLS is that QR is more robust to non-normal errors and outliers (Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017; Hübler, 2017; Moutinho et al., 2017; Niemierko et al., 2019; Salman et al., 2019; Zietz et al., 2008). Additionally, QR is less sensitive to heteroscedasticity since the standard errors of the coefficients are estimated by bootstrapping, and eliminates distributional assumptions (Hübler, 2017; Moutinho et al., 2017; Salman et al., 2019; Zietz et al., 2008). Niemierko et al. (2019) stated that having different regression line slopes at different quantiles also helps with heteroscedasticity.

For this study, preliminary analysis revealed that the OLS model suffered from heteroscedasticity and, thus, QR analysis was conducted to determine the behavior of the SCI at different quantiles in relation to the independent variables. Defining quantiles for research depends on the researchers’ objectives. However, quantile regressions are common (Buchinsky, 2008; Eide & Showalter, 2007; Zietz et al., 2008). Other QR intervals used by researchers include quantiles in 0.05 intervals (Hübler, 2017; Wang et al., 2019), 0.10 intervals (Salman et al., 2019; Zietz et al., 2008), 0.2 intervals (Niemierko et al., 2019), and unequal spreads (Gallego-Álvarez & Ortas, 2017; Tan & Wang, 2017; Xu et al., 2017). For this study, quantiles at every 0.1 interval were used in the analysis. The QR analysis was done using SPSS.

SPSS does not provide pooled results for the QR analysis with imputed datasets. After consulting with statistician S. Roy (personal communication, January 12, 2021) simple averages of the ten imputations were recommended as a pseudo pooled result. Average calculations were applied to R2 per quantile, R2 across all quantiles, parameter effects per quantile, and p-values per quantile. Trendline analysis was also conducted when a variable was significant in five or more quantiles. This allowed for reporting the average change in a variable per quantile.

4. Results

The SCI variable had a mean of 2.024 based on the pooled results of the ten multiple imputations (Table 1). Standard deviations for the SCI ranged from 0.719 (imputation 8) to 0.740 (imputation 9), with an average of 0.729. Descriptive statistics for the indexed and continuous variables are presented in Table 1. SCI scores, frequencies and percentages are presented in Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the independent variables are presented in Table 3.

Table 1. Descriptive data for indexed and continuous variables used in the QR analysis. Represents the average across imputations.

Note: Data in this table is based on the pooled results for the descriptive output.

Table 2. SCI scores, frequencies, and percentages based on the average across imputations.

Table 3. Descriptive data for variables used in the QR analysis.

Note: Data in this table is based on the pooled results for the descriptive output.

Predictors of Social Communication on Climate Change

The average R2 across all imputations was 0.292, where the lowest average R2 per quantile was 0.259 (0.1 quantile), and the highest average R2 per quantile was 0.326 (0.8 quantile) (see Table 4). Results of the quantile regression showed that Knowledge, age, income, newspaper use, urbanicity, and race significantly affected the SCI in one or more quantiles. Social media, sex, and religiosity were insignificant throughout all quantiles. Newspaper use was significant across all quantiles, and parameter estimates showed that not using newspapers reduced the SCI by an average of 0.603. P-values ranged from <0.001 to 0.007. Trendline analysis showed that the parameter estimates for newspapers reduced by an average of 0.114 between quantiles. Across all quantiles, the newspaper variable had a negative effect on SCI, where the largest impact occurred in the 0.8 quantile (−0.927, p-value ≤ 0.001), and the smallest impact occurred in the 0.1 quantile (−0.312, p-value ≤ 0.001).

Knowledge was significant across all quantiles except the 0.4 and 0.5 quantiles.

Knowledge had a positive impact on the SCI throughout all significant quantiles, where the average impact was 0.871, the largest impact occurred in the 0.9

Table 4. QR results: parameter estimates and p-values of model variables across quantiles. Pseudo R2 is represented in last row.

Note: White and rural are the reference categories for race and residence variables, respectively. Figures represent averages across 10 imputations. * = significant parameter estimates, p-values appear in brackets (<0.05), Pseudo R2 is represented for each quantile in the last row.

quantile (1.497, p-value = 0.042), and the smallest impact occurred in the 0.3 quantile (0.598, p-value = 0.029). P-values ranged from <0.005 to 0.050. Trendline analysis of the significant quantiles revealed that Knowledge increased the SCI by an average 0.200 between quantiles. Age was significant for quantiles 0.1 through 0.5, where the parameter estimates were −0.018 (p-value ≤ 0.001), −0.014 (p-value ≤ 0.001), −0.011 (p-value = 0.024), −0.013 (p-value = 0.020), and −0.012 (p-value = 0.045) respectively. Income was only significant at the 0.2 quantile, where the parameter estimate was −0.105 (p-value = 0.028).

Collectively, Race was significant for quantiles 0.3 through 0.9. Individual races affected the SCI differently. Being Black (in comparison to the reference category of white) did not impact the SCI at any quantile. Being Asian negatively affected the SCI from quantiles 0.3 through 0.8. The average impact of the Asian race was −0.833, where the highest impact occurred at quantile 0.5 (−0.995, p-value ≤ 0.001), and the lowest impact occurred at the 0.3 quantile (−0.474, p-value = 0.050). Similarly, income was only significant at the 0.7 quantile, where the impact on SCI was −0.132 (p-value ≤ 0.001). Religiosity was significant at quantiles 0.1 and 0.2, where the parameter estimates were 0.165 (p-value ≤ 0.001) and 0.108 (p-value = 0.020). The Urbanicity variables collectively affected the SCI at quartiles 0.4 through 0.9. The suburban category was significant at quantiles 0.4 through 0.7, where the SCI was higher by 0.336 (p-value = 0.044), 0.613 (p-value = 0.004), 0.672 (p-value = 0.001), and 0.574 (p-value < 0.001), respectively, relative to rural residents. The urban category was significant at quantiles 0.7, 0.8, and 0.9, where the impact to the SCI was by 0.461 (p-value < 0.001), 0.415 (p-value = 0.003), and 0.497 (p-value = 0.046).

The slope of the parameter prediction lines per quantile was generally associated with the average impact on SCI. For example, the parameter prediction lines for knowledge were upward sloping for all the quantiles, even though some gradients were steeper than others. The slopes of the prediction lines also changed across imputations but followed the trend (positive or negative).

5. Discussion & Conclusion

The QR analysis revealed that newspaper usage was significant across all quantiles where using newspapers increased the SCI. The parameter estimates for newspaper use generally decreased until the 0.7 percentile, after which the values began to increase. This finding aligns with previous findings by other researchers like Anderson (2011), Devkota and Phuyal (2018), and Duan et al. (2017) on newspapers being important for CCC. For the current study, newspaper use was more important for more active communicators on climate change than less active communicators. Knowledge was significant across most quantiles and had the largest average impact on the SCI. Knowledge on climate change was identified as an important element of climate change communication (Ballantyne, 2016; Chadwick, 2018; Devkota & Phuyal, 2018; Nerlich et al., 2010; Ouariachi et al., 2017). However, the sharp increase (1.497 SCI points) at the 0.9 quantile suggested that knowledge on climate change influences communication at higher levels of socialization. Nevertheless, knowledge had an average impact on the SCI of 0.871, at least 25% of a respondent’s social communication score, considering the maximum SCI score was 3.5. Trendline analysis showed that knowledge increased by 0.200 between quantiles, indicating that as average knowledge increased, the SCI increased.

Age was significant at the lower levels of SCI (quantiles 0.1 through 0.5) and had a negative impact. The significance at the lower levels of social communication aligns with research that suggests older individuals are less engaged with climate change (Arbuckle et al., 2017; Detenber et al., 2016; Leiserowitz et al., 2021). However, some researchers stated that younger individuals, who are likely to be more impacted, contribute more to climate change discussions (Ortega-Egea et al., 2014), while others stated that older individuals with more knowledge on the issue may communicate more (Habtemariam et al., 2016). As for this study, based on the distribution of the Florida population, the assertions by authors like Leiserowitz et al. (2021). When compared to White individuals, Black communities do not significantly share any information about climate change. This contradicts Leiserowitz et al. (2021) who found that more than 50% of African Americans were concerned or alarmed. However, Asian individuals communicate significantly less when compared to White individuals throughout most of the SCI (quantiles 0.3 to 0.8). Additionally, other races share significantly at the higher levels of social communication. Previous research highlighted that White individuals are less likely to be concerned with the environment or climate change when compared to other races, where socioeconomic status and economic inequalities as some reasons (Pearson et al., 2017; Schuldt & Pearson, 2016). Further, other studies found race to be significant in relation to climate change issues, vulnerabilities to climate change regulations, and adaptability were concerns (Azong & Kelso, 2021; Hansen et al., 2013; Semenza et al., 2008).

In previous studies, women were reported to be more involved in climate change issues than men (Chadwick, 2018; Holmberg & Hellsten, 2015; McCright, 2010; Scannell & Gifford, 2013). However, sex was not significant at any quantile in the current study. Compared to being from a rural area, being from an urban area was significant at the higher levels of SCI, where a positive impact was observed. Being from a suburban area was also significant at the mid-range of the quantiles (0.4 to 0.7) and had a positive impact when compared to individuals from a rural area. Nevertheless, the location of the respondent had substantial influence on social communication on climate change, which also aligns with previous research (Jenkins et al., 2018). Income was only significant at the 0.2 quantile. Individuals with higher income were identified to spend more on climate mitigation strategies and may be also talking more about the issue (Rahu & Ali, 2017). However, income had a negative effect on the SCI, which contradict those results.

Climate change communication on social media was researched by various authors (Bennett et al., 2021; Harris et al., 2016; Lorenzon et al., 2021; Rahman et al., 2021). The results of the current study suggest that using social media does not impact social communication on climate change in Florida. Some authors identified that social media presents a space for articulating, identifying, and experiencing for various views on climate change which can transition from an online presence to a physical presence (Anderson & Huntington, 2017; Shearer & Grieco, 2019). However, the literature describes communicating online is different than communicating in person. Further investigation into how social media impacts social communication should be conducted as per the contemporary social media market.

In conclusion, the Social Communication Index was created to determine the demographic characteristics of Florida residents who were likely to communicate about climate change. By using a quantile regression analysis, the SCI was tested against eight demographic variables across nine quantiles in 0.1 intervals. Regarding O1, the results of the study showed that knowledge, age, income, newspaper use, race, and urbanicity were significant in, at least, one quantile. Religiosity, sex, and social media use were insignificant across all quantiles in this model. Pervious research is consistent with most of the results. However, for the variables that were insignificant across all quantiles, further investigation is warranted as previous research differs from these results.

Regarding O2, the results showed that, at different significant quantiles, variables had different impacts on the SCI. For variables that allowed for trendline analysis, the average change from quantile to quantile was sharp. However, some variables showed parameter effects in the quantiles that had no pattern, while others showed peaking around the 0.5 quantile. As mentioned, this is an advantage of QR: gaining a better understanding of the effects at different levels of social communication. The results of this study can add to the body of research on climate change CCC, but with the focus on social communication. Additionally, adding more variables that are common across the literature, like other forms of media, education, and political affiliation, can provide more robust results. The current study also is also focused on Florida. Replicating the study across other states may confirm the current results. Finally, many researchers compare results of an OLS model to a QR model. Future research can focus on examining a functional OLS, and comparing that to a QR model for SCI.

This research adds to the body of knowledge concerning CCC in a unique way: creating a variable that focuses on social communication among individuals. The results of the current study can inform the direction communicators, like campaigners, journalists, and policy makers, as they attempt to target different audiences with specific climate change information. Specifically for policy makers, this research highlights the audience segment that can assist with accessing and spreading the right information about climate change. Additionally, this research adds to the literature that presents climate communication as a social process, similar to Ballantyne (2016). Moving forward, collecting new data for current trends in social communication on climate change would inform future needs of social communicators.

Limitations

The sample size obtained for the study was smaller than planned and this reduced the likelihood of finding significant but weak effects for some demographics. In addition, the low response rate can result in bias for variables not included in the weighting procedures (Biemer & Christ, 2008). The SCI variable used a simple average approach on questions that varied on the scales used (three-point to five-point). A weighted average approach may have allowed for each question to have the same weight in the SCI variable. Another limitation of the study concerns the variables used. Most of the variables were demographic variables coupled with a few other variable types. Focusing more closely on communication variables may have been beneficial in the analysis. Finally, the data is from 2016-2017 and with recent events, climate change experiences and opinions, as well as communication practices may have shifted.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

References

[1] AAPOR (2016). Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. American Association for Public Opinion Research [AAPOR].
http://www.aapor.org/AAPOR_Main/media/publications/Standard-Definitions20169theditionfinal.pdf
[2] Aksit, O., McNeal, K. S., Gold, A. U., Libarkin, J. C., & Harris, S. (2018). The Influence of Instruction, Prior Knowledge, and Values on Climate Change Risk Perception among Undergraduates. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 55, 550-572.
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21430
[3] Anderson, A. (2011). Sources, Media, and Modes of Climate Change Communication: The Role of Celebrities. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 2, 535-546.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.119
[4] Anderson, A. A., & Huntington, H. E. (2017). Social Media, Science, and Attack Discourse: How Twitter Discussions of Climate Change Use Sarcasm and Incivility. Science Communication, 39, 598-620.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547017735113
[5] Arbuckle, J. G., Tyndall, J. C., Morton, L. W., & Hobbs, J. (2017). Climate Change Typologies and Audience Segmentation among Corn Belt Farmers. Journal of Soil and Water Conservation, 72, 205-214.
https://doi.org/10.2489/jswc.72.3.205
[6] Atzori, R., Fyall, A., & Miller, G. (2018). Tourist Responses to Climate Change: Potential Impacts and Adaptation in Florida’s Coastal Destinations. Tourism Management, 69, 12-22.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2018.05.005
[7] Azong, M. N., & Kelso, C. J. (2021). Gender, Ethnicity and Vulnerability to Climate Change: The Case of Matrilineal and Patrilineal Societies in Bamenda Highlands Region, Cameroon. Global Environmental Change, 67, Article ID: 102241.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2021.102241
[8] Badullovich, N., Grant, W. J., & Colvin, R. M. (2020). Framing Climate Change for Effective Communication: A Systematic Map. Environmental Research Letters, 15, Article ID: 123002.
https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aba4c7
[9] Ballantyne, A. G. (2016). Climate Change Communication: What Can We Learn from Communication Theory? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7, 329-344.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.392
[10] Barkemeyer, R., Figge, F., Hoepner, A., Holt, D., Kraak, J. M., & Yu, P.-S. (2017). Media Coverage of Climate Change: An International Comparison. Environment and Planning C: Politics and Space, 35, 1029-1054.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0263774X16680818
[11] Bennett, J., Rachunok, B., Flage, R., & Nateghi, R. (2021). Mapping Climate Discourse to Climate Opinion: An Approach for Augmenting Surveys with Social Media to Enhance Understandings of Climate Opinion in the United States. PLoS ONE, 16, e0245319.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245319
[12] Berglund, P. A. (2010). An Introduction to Multiple Imputation of Complex Sample Data Using SAS v9.2. In SAS Global Forum (pp. 1-12).
http://support.sas.com/resources/papers/proceedings10/265-2010.pdf
[13] Bertolotti, M., & Catellani, P. (2014). Effects of Message Framing in Policy Communication on Climate Change. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 474-486.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2033
[14] Biemer, P. P., & Christ, S. L. (2008). Weighting Survey Data. In International Handbook of Survey Design (pp. 317-341). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[15] Bolsen, T., & Shapiro, M. A. (2018). The US News Media, Polarization on Climate Change, and Pathways to Effective Communication. Environmental Communication, 12, 149-163.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1397039
[16] Boulianne, S., Lalancette, M., & Ilkiw, D. (2020). “School Strike 4 Climate”: Social Media and the International Youth Protest on Climate Change. Media and Communication, 8, 208-218.
https://doi.org/10.17645/mac.v8i2.2768
[17] Boussalis, C., Coan, T. G., & Holman, M. R. (2018). Climate Change Communication from Cities in the USA. Climatic Change, 149, 173-187.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-018-2223-1
[18] Bowen, M. S. (2008). Censoring Science: Inside the Political Attack on Dr. James Hansen and the Truth of Global Warming. Penguin.
[19] Brügger, A. (2020). Understanding the Psychological Distance of Climate Change: The Limitations of Construal Level Theory and Suggestions for Alternative Theoretical Perspectives. Global Environmental Change, 60, Article ID: 102023.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2019.102023
[20] Buchinsky, M. (2008). Quantile Regression. The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, 15, 787-793.
https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-349-95121-5_2795-1
[21] Chadwick, A. E. (2018). Climate Change Communication: Challenges, Insights, and Opportunities. In Psychology and Climate Change: Human Perceptions, Impacts, and Responses (pp. 35-63). Elsevier.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-813130-5.00003-5
[22] Colombo, E. (2017). Enforcing International Climate Change Law in Domestic Courts: A New Trend of Cases for Boosting Principle 10 of the Rio Declaration. UCLA Journal of Environmental Law & Policy, 35, 98-144.
https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7g98s7f3
https://doi.org/10.5070/L5351034672
[23] Detenber, B., Rosenthal, S., Liao, Y., & Ho, S. S. (2016). Audience Segmentation for Campaign Design: Addressing Climate Change in Singapore. International Journal of Communication, 10, 4736-4758.
https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/4696/1797
[24] Devkota, N., & Phuyal, R. K. (2018). Adoption Practice of Climate Change Adaptation Options among Nepalese Rice Farmers: Role of Information and Communication Technologies (ICTs). American Journal of Climate Change, 7, 135-152.
https://doi.org/10.4236/ajcc.2018.72010
[25] Dillman, D. A., Smyth, J. D., & Christian, L. M. (2014). Internet, Phone, Mail, and Mixed-Mode Surveys: The Tailored Design Method. John Wiley & Sons.
[26] Duan, R., Zwickle, A., & Takahashi, B. (2017). A Construal-Level Perspective of Climate Change Images in US Newspapers. Climatic Change, 142, 345-360.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-017-1945-9
[27] Duan, R., Zwickle, A., & Takahashi, B. (2021). Refining the Application of Construal Level Theory: Egocentric and Nonegocentric Psychological Distances in Climate Change Visual Communication. Environmental Communication, 1-16.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2021.1964999
[28] Ecklund, E. H., Scheitle, C. P., Peifer, J., & Bolger, D. (2017). Examining Links between Religion, Evolution Views, and Climate Change Skepticism. Environment and Behavior, 49, 985-1006.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916516674246
[29] Eide, E., & Showalter, M. H. (2007). Regression Approach. Stock Market Anomalies, 58, 129-157.
[30] Elia, E., Mutula, S., & Stilwell, C. (2015). Disseminating and Using Information on Climate Change and Variability: A Case Study of Farmers in Maluga and Chibela Villages in Central Tanzania. Mousaion, 33, 1-24.
https://doi.org/10.25159/0027-2639/157
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=llf&AN=114722451&site=ehost-live
[31] Erni, B., Navarini, A. A., Huang, D., Schoetzau, A., Kind, A., & Mueller, S. M. (2021). Proposition of a Severity Scale for Lichen Sclerosus: The “Clinical Lichen Sclerosus Score.” Dermatologic Therapy, 34, e14773.
https://doi.org/10.1111/dth.14773
[32] Field, C. B. (2018). Smart Adaptation in an Era of Rising Climate Risks. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists, 74, 73-80.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00963402.2018.1436806
[33] Forchtner, B., Kroneder, A., & Wetzel, D. (2018). Being Skeptical? Exploring Far-Right Climate-Change Communication in Germany. Environmental Communication, 12, 589-604.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2018.1470546
[34] Gadoth, A., Halbrook, M., Martin-Blais, R., Gray, A., Tobin, N. H., Ferbas, K. G., Aldrovandi, G. M., & Rimoin, A. W. (2021). Cross-Sectional Assessment of COVID-19 Vaccine Acceptance among Health Care Workers in Los Angeles. Annals of Internal Medicine, 174, 882-885.
https://doi.org/10.7326/M20-7580
[35] Gallego-álvarez, I., & Ortas, E. (2017). Corporate Environmental Sustainability Reporting in the Context of National Cultures: A Quantile Regression Approach. International Business Review, 26, 337-353.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibusrev.2016.09.003
[36] Garas, M., Vaccarezza, M., Newland, G., McVay-Doornbusch, K., & Hasani, J. (2018). 3D-Printed Specimens as a Valuable Tool in Anatomy Education: A Pilot Study. Annals of Anatomy-Anatomischer Anzeiger, 219, 57-64.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aanat.2018.05.006
[37] Habtemariam, L. T., Gandorfer, M., Kassa, G. A., & Heissenhuber, A. (2016). Factors Influencing Smallholder Farmers’ Climate Change Perceptions: A Study from Farmers in Ethiopia. Environmental Management, 58, 343-358.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00267-016-0708-0
[38] Hansen, A., Bi, L., Saniotis, A., & Nitschke, M. (2013). Vulnerability to Extreme Heat and Climate Change: Is Ethnicity a Factor? Global Health Action, 6, 1-7.
https://doi.org/10.3402/gha.v6i0.21364
[39] Harris, U., Papoutsaki, E., & Kailahi, S. (2016). ICTs in Climate Change Communication in the Pacific Islands. Information Technologies & International Development, 12, 47-58.
https://itidjournal.org/index.php/itid/article/view/1569/561
[40] Hine, D. W., Phillips, W. J., Driver, A. B., & Morrison, M. (2017). Audience Segmentation and Climate Change Communication. In M. Nisbet (Ed.), Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science (pp. 66-94). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.390
[41] Hine, D. W., Reser, J. P., Morrison, M., Phillips, W. J., Nunn, P., & Cooksey, R. (2014). Audience Segmentation and Climate Change Communication: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 5, 441-459.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.279
[42] Holmberg, K., & Hellsten, I. (2015). Gender Differences in the Climate Change Communication on Twitter. Internet Research, 25, 811-828.
https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-07-2014-0179
[43] Huber, S., & Huber, O. W. (2012). The Centrality of Religiosity Scale (CRS). Religions, 3, 710-724.
[44] Hübler, M. (2017). The Inequality-Emissions Nexus in the Context of Trade and Development: A Quantile Regression Approach. Ecological Economics, 134, 174-185.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2016.12.015
[45] Jenkins, W., Berry, E., & Kreider, L. B. (2018). Religion and Climate Change. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 43, 85-108.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-102017-025855
[46] Johnson, B. B. (2012). Climate Change Communication: A Provocative Inquiry into Motives, Meanings, and Means. Risk Analysis, 32, 973-991.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01731.x
[47] Jones, C., Hine, D. W., & Marks, A. D. G. (2017). The Future Is Now: Reducing Psychological Distance to Increase Public Engagement with Climate Change. Risk Analysis, 37, 331-341.
https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12601
[48] Khadka, A., Li, C. J., Stanis, S. W., & Morgan, M. (2021). Unpacking the Power of Place-Based Education in Climate Change Communication. Applied Environmental Education and Communication, 20, 77-91.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1533015X.2020.1719238
[49] Koenker, R. W., & Bassett, G. (1978). Regression Quantiles. Econometrica, 46, 33-50.
[50] Koenker, R., & Hallock, K. F. (2001). Quantile Regression. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 15, 143-156.
https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.15.4.143
[51] Korten, T. (2015, March 19). In Florida, Officials Ban Term “Climate Change”. Miami Herald.
https://fcir.org/2015/03/08/in-florida-officials-ban-term-climate-change
[52] Kunelius, R., Eide, E., Tegelberg, M., & Yagodin, D. (2017). Media and Global Climate Knowledge. Springer.
https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-52321-1
[53] Layfield, E., Triantafillou, V., Prasad, A., Deng, J., Shanti, R. M., Newman, J. G., & Rajasekaran, K. (2020). Telemedicine for Head and Neck Ambulatory Visits during COVID-19: Evaluating Usability and Patient Satisfaction. Head & Neck, 42, 1681-1689.
https://doi.org/10.1002/hed.26285
[54] Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., & Rosenthal, S. (2015). Climate Change in the American Mind. Annals of Global Health, 81, 396-409.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aogh.2015.08.010
[55] Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., Marlon, J., & Maibach, E. (2021). Global Warming’s Six Americas: A Review and Recommendations for Climate Change Communication. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 97-103.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.04.007
[56] Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. (2010). Americans’ Knowledge of Climate Change. Yale University.
https://climatecommunication.yale.edu/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/2010_10_Americans’-Knowledge-of-Climate-Change.pdf
[57] Lindsey, R., Herring, D., & Greenhalgh, E. (2018). Response by Lindsey, Herring, and Greenhalgh to “A Missed Opportunity? NOAA’s Use of Social Media to Communicate Climate Science”. Environmental Communication, 12, 284-286.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1394659
[58] Lorenzen, K., Ainsworth, C. H., Baker, S. M., Barbieri, L. R., Camp, E. V, Dotson, J. R., & Lester, S. E. (2017). Climate Change Impacts on Florida’s Fisheries and Aquaculture Sectors and Options for Adaptation. In Florida’s Climate: Changes, Variations, & Impacts (pp. 427-454). Florida Climate Change Institute.
http://purl.flvc.org/fsu/fd/FSU_libsubv1_scholarship_submission_1515510950_127e8200
https://doi.org/10.17125/fci2017.ch14
[59] Lorenzon, L., Grossman, R. C., & Soreide, K. (2021). Impact of Randomized Controlled Trials in the Social Media: Does Science Trend as Much as Everyday Events? World Journal of Surgery, 45, 88-96.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00268-020-05769-8
[60] Loy, L. S., & Spence, A. (2020). Reducing, and Bridging, the Psychological Distance of Climate Change. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 67, 1-8.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101388
[61] Maibach, E. W., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., & Mertz, C. K. (2011). Identifying Like-Minded Audiences for Global Warming Public Engagement Campaigns: An Audience Segmentation Analysis and Tool Development. PLOS ONE, 6, e17571.
[62] Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz., A. (2009). Global Warming’s Six Americas 2009: An Audience Segmentation Analysis. Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.
[63] McAlpine, S. A., & Porter, J. R. (2018). Estimating Recent Local Impacts of Sea-Level Rise on Current Real-Estate Losses: A Housing Market Case Study in Miami-Dade, Florida. Population Research and Policy Review, 37, 871-895.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11113-018-9473-5
[64] McCright, A. M. (2010). The Effects of Gender on Climate Change Knowledge and Concern in the American Public. Population and Environment, 32, 66-87.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11111-010-0113-1
[65] Middelbeek, L., Kolle, K., & Verrest, H. (2014). Built to Last? Local Climate Change Adaptation and Governance in the Caribbean—The Case of an Informal Urban Settlement in Trinidad and Tobago. Urban Climate, 8, 138-154.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.uclim.2013.12.003
[66] Morrison, M., Parton, K., & Hine, D. W. (2018). Increasing Belief but Issue Fatigue: Changes in Australian Household Climate Change Segments between 2011 and 2016. PLOS ONE, 13, e0197988.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0197988
[67] Moutinho, V., Madaleno, M., & Robaina, M. (2017). The Economic and Environmental Efficiency Assessment in EU Cross-Country: Evidence from DEA and Quantile Regression Approach. Ecological Indicators, 78, 85-97.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2017.02.042
[68] Nerlich, B., Koteyko, N., & Brown, B. (2010). Theory and Language of Climate Change Communication. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 1, 97-110.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.2
[69] Niemierko, R., Töppel, J., & Tränkler, T. (2019). A D-Vine Copula Quantile Regression Approach for the Prediction of Residential Heating Energy Consumption Based on Historical Data. Applied Energy, 233-234, 691-708.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apenergy.2018.10.025
[70] NOAA National Centers for Environmental Information (NCEI) U.S. Billion-Dollar Weather and Climate Disasters (2022).
https://www.ncdc.noaa.gov/billions
[71] Ortega-Egea, J. M., García-de-Frutos, N., & Antolín-López, R. (2014). Why Do Some People Do “More” to Mitigate Climate Change than Others? Exploring Heterogeneity in Psycho-Social Associations. PLoS ONE, 9, e106645.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106645
[72] Ouariachi, T., Olvera-Lobo, M. D., & Gutiérrez-Pérez, J. (2017). Analyzing Climate Change Communication through Online Games: Development and Application of Validated Criteria. Science Communication, 39, 10-44.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547016687998
[73] Paukert, C. P., Glazer, B. A., Hansen, G. J. A., Irwin, B. J., Jacobson, P. C., Kershner, J. L., Shuter, B. J., Whitney, J. E., & Lynch, A. J. (2016). Adapting Inland Fisheries Management to a Changing Climate. Fisheries, 41, 374-384.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03632415.2016.1185009
[74] Pearson, A. R., Ballew, M. T., Naiman, S., Schuldt, J. P., Pearson, A. R., Ballew, M. T., Naiman, S., & Schuldt, J. P. (2017). Race, Class, Gender and Climate Change Communication. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science (Issue January, pp. 1-32). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.412
[75] Pong, V. (2021). Global versus Local Framing of the Issue of Food Waste: The Role of Identification with All Humanity and the Implications for Climate Change Communication. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 221-231.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12453
[76] Rahman, M. S., Karamehic-Muratovic, A., Baghbanzadeh, M., Amrin, M., Zafar, S., Rahman, N. N., Shirina, S. U., & Haque, U. (2021). Climate Change and Dengue Fever Knowledge, Attitudes and Practices in Bangladesh: A social Media-Based Cross-Sectional Survey. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene, 115, 85-93.
https://doi.org/10.1093/trstmh/traa093
[77] Rahu, D. B., & Ali, A. (2017). Coping with Climate Change and Its Impact on Productivity, Income, and Poverty: Evidence from the Himalayan Region of Pakistan. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, 24, 515-525.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2017.05.006
[78] Robbins, D. (2020). Climate Change and the Media. In Ireland and the Climate Crisis (pp. 169-183). Palgrave Macmillan.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-47587-1_10
[79] Romsdahl, R. J. (2020). Deliberative Framing: Opening Up Discussions for Local-Level Public Engagement on Climate Change. Climatic Change, 162, 145-163.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-020-02754-x
[80] Russill, C. (2008). Tipping Point Forewarnings in Climate Change Communication: Some Implications of an Emerging Trend. Environmental Communication, 2, 133-153.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524030802141711
[81] Russill, C., & Nyssa, Z. (2009). The Tipping Point Trend in Climate Change Communication. Global Environmental Change, 19, 336-344.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2009.04.001
[82] Sakellari, M. (2015). Cinematic Climate Change, a Promising Perspective on Climate Change Communication. Public Understanding of Science, 24, 827-841.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662514537028
[83] Salman, M., Long, X., Dauda, L., Mensah, C. N., & Muhammad, S. (2019). Different Impacts of Export and Import on Carbon Emissions across 7 ASEAN Countries: A Panel Quantile Regression Approach. Science of the Total Environment, 686, 1019-1029.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.06.019
[84] Saudek, K., Saudek, D., Treat, R., Bartz, P., Weigert, R., & Weisgerber, M. (2018). Dear Program Director: Deciphering Letters of Recommendation. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 10, 261-266.
https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-17-00712.1
[85] Scannell, L., & Gifford, R. (2013). Personally Relevant Climate Change: The Role of Place Attachment and Local versus Global Message Framing in Engagement. Environment and Behavior, 45, 60-85.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916511421196
[86] Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing Data: Our View of the State of the Art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147-177.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147
[87] Schuldt, J. P., & Pearson, A. R. (2016). The Role of Race and Ethnicity in Climate Change Polarization: Evidence from a US National Survey Experiment. Climatic Change, 136, 495-505.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-016-1631-3
[88] Schuldt, J. P., Rickard, L. N., & Yang, Z. J. (2018). Does Reduced Psychological Distance Increase Climate Engagement? On the Limits of Localizing Climate Change. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 55, 147-153.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.02.001
[89] Semenza, J. C., Hall, D. E., Wilson, D. J., Bontempo, B. D., Sailor, D. J., & George, L. A. (2008). Public Perception of Climate Change: Voluntary Mitigation and Barriers to Behavior Change. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35, 479-487.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2008.08.020
[90] Shearer, E., & Grieco, E. (2019). Americans Are Wary of the Role Social Media Sites Play in Delivering the News. Pew Research Center.
https://mth101.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/10/FS19_Midterm-Articles.pdf
[91] Shen, S., Feng, X., & Peng, Z. R. (2016). A Framework to Analyze Vulnerability of Critical Infrastructure to Climate Change: The Case of a Coastal Community in Florida. Natural Hazards, 84, 589-609.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-016-2442-6
[92] Stanton, E. A., & Ackerman, F. (2007). Florida and Climate Change: The Costs of Inaction. In Florida and Climate Change: The Costs of Inaction. Global Development and Environment Institute, Tufts University.
https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abstract/20083154838
[93] Stevenson, K. T., King, T. L., Selm, K. R., Peterson, M. N., & Monroe, M. C. (2018). Framing Climate Change Communication to Prompt Individual and Collective Action among Adolescents from Agricultural Communities. Environmental Education Research, 24, 365-377.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2017.1318114
[94] Tàbara, J. D., Clair, A. L. S., & Hermansen, E. A. T. (2017). Transforming Communication and Knowledge Production Processes to Address High-End Climate Change. Environmental Science & Policy, 70, 31-37.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envsci.2017.01.004
[95] Tan, X. P., & Wang, X. Y. (2017). Dependence Changes between the Carbon Price and Its Fundamentals: A Quantile Regression Approach. Applied Energy, 190, 306-325.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apenergy.2016.12.116
[96] Thomas, K., Hardy, R. D., Lazrus, H., Mendez, M., Orlove, B., Rivera-Collazo, I., Roberts, J. T., Rockman, M., Warner, B. P., & Winthrop, R. (2019). Explaining Differential Vulnerability to Climate Change: A Social Science Review. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 10, 1-18.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.565
[97] Trenberth, K. E., Cheng, L., Jacobs, P., Zhang, Y., & Fasullo, J. (2018). Hurricane Harvey Links to Ocean Heat Content and Climate Change Adaptation. Earth’s Future, 6, 730-744.
https://doi.org/10.1029/2018EF000825
[98] Van der Hel, S., Hellsten, I., & Steen, G. (2018). Tipping Points and Climate Change: Metaphor between Science and the Media. Environmental Communication, 12, 605-620.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2017.1410198
[99] Wang, S. J., Zeng, J., & Liu, X. (2019). Examining the Multiple Impacts of Technological Progress on CO2 Emissions in China: A Panel Quantile Regression Approach. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews, 103, 140-150.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rser.2018.12.046
[100] Wang, S., Corner, A., Chapman, D., & Markowitz, E. (2018). Public Engagement with Climate Imagery in a Changing Digital Landscape. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 9, 1-18.
https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.509
[101] Wibeck, V. (2014). Enhancing Learning, Communication and Public Engagement about Climate Change—Some Lessons from Recent Literature. Environmental Education Research, 20, 387-411.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2013.812720
[102] Wint, N. (2019). Wet/Hurricane Season Preparedness Guide. Office of Disaster Preparedness and Management.
http://www.odpm.gov.tt/sites/default/files/2019ODPMHURRICANEPREPAREDNESSGUIDEFINAL%281%29-compressed.pdf
[103] Wonneberger, A., Meijers, M. H. C., & Schuck, A. R. T. (2020). Shifting Public Engagement: How Media Coverage of Climate Change Conferences Affects Climate Change Audience Segments. Public Understanding of Science, 29, 176-193.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662519886474
[104] Woodward, A. J., & Samet, J. M. (2018). Climate Change, Hurricanes, and Health. American Journal of Public Health, 108, 33-35.
https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2017.304197
[105] Wulandari, C., & Inoue, M. (2018). The Importance of Social Learning for the Development of Community Based Forest Management in Indonesia: The Case of Community Forestry in Lampung Province. Small-Scale Forestry, 17, 361-376.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11842-018-9392-7
[106] Xu, R., Xu, L., & Xu, B. (2017). Assessing CO2 Emissions in China’s Iron and Steel Industry: Evidence from Quantile Regression Approach. Journal of Cleaner Production, 152, 259-270.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2017.03.142
[107] Zietz, J., Zietz, E. N., & Sirmans, G. S. (2008). Determinants of House Prices: A Quantile Regression Approach. Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, 37, 317-333.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11146-007-9053-7

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.