
M. S. WALID ET AL.9
from previous fusion attempts. Atlas-axis fusions also
increase in older-old patients due to a comparatively
elevated risk of upper cervical spine injuries in elderly
patients [11]. Female old-old patients unsurprisingly
present with higher frequency of vertebral fracture acci-
dents and therefore require more such repairs due to
higher prevalence of osteoporosis [7]. The relatively
higher occurrence of dorsal/dorsolumbar fusions and
spine structure repair operations in the <40 age group
could be related to more frequent traumatic and congeni-
tal etiology in younger age. The preponderance of fe-
males patients in the later three age groups and males in
the younger group reflects the fact that younger males
are subject to suffer more professional spine injuries
while females have better longevity and the longer they
live the higher the risk of osteoporosis.
In summary, the spine surgery profile of our patients
shows a tendency toward less invasive procedures in the
older-old population unless indicated by previous sur-
gery failures, upper neck injuries or osteoporosis-induced
fractures.
5. References
[1] Population in the U.S. Google Public Data Explorer.
[2] P. B. Baltes and J. Smith, “New Frontiers in the Future of
Aging: From Successful Aging of the Young Old to the
Dilemmas of the Fourth Age,” Gerontology, Vol. 49, No.
2, 2003, pp. 123-135. doi:10.1159/000067946
[3] R. Rozzini, I. Sleiman, S. Maggi, M. Noale and M. Tra-
bucchi, “Gender Differences and Health Status in Old and
Very Old Patients,” Journal of the American Medical
Directors Association, Vol. 10, No. 8, 2009, pp. 554-558.
doi:10.1016/j.jamda.2009.04.005
[4] J. Cohen-Mansfield, D. Shmotkin, N. Eyal, Y. Reichental
and H. Hazan, “A Comparison of Three Types of Auto-
biographical Memories in Old-Old Age: First Memories,
Pivotal Memories and Traumatic Memories,” Gerontol-
ogy, Vol. 56, No. 6, 2010, pp. 564-573.
doi:10.1159/000295113
[5] D. Paulson, M. E. Bowen and P. A. Lichtenberg, “Suc-
cessful Aging and Longevity in Older Old Women: The
Role of Depression and Cognition,” Journal of Aging
Research, 2011.
[6] D. McGinnis and E. M. Zelinski, “Understanding Unfa-
miliar Words in Young, Young-Old, and Old-Old Adults:
Inferential Processing and the Abstraction-Deficit Hy-
pothesis,” Psychol Aging, Vol. 18, No. 3, 2003, pp.
497-509. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.497
[7] C. A. Inderjeeth, A. C. Foo, M. M. Lai and P. Glen-
denning, “Efficacy and Safety of Pharmacological Agents
in Managing Osteoporosis in the Old-Old: Review of the
Evidence,” Bone, Vol. 44, No. 5, 2009, pp. 744-751.
doi:10.1016/j.bone.2008.12.003
[8] K. Zinke, M. Zeintl, A. Eschen, C. Herzog and M.
Kliegel, “Potentials and Limits of Plasticity Induced by
Working Memory Training in Old-Old Age,” Gerontol-
ogy, 2011.
[9] B. Draper and L. F. Low, “Psychiatric Services for the
Old-Old,” International Psychogeriatric Association, Vol.
22, No. 4, 2010, pp. 582-588.
[10] F. Landi, A. Russo, R. Liperoti, P. Danese, E. Maiorana,
M. Pahor, R. Bernabei and G. Onder, “Daily Pain and
Functional Decline Among Old-Old Adults Living in the
Community: Results from the ilSIRENTE Study,” Jour-
nal of Pain Symptom Manage, Vol. 38, No. 3, 2009, pp.
350-357. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2008.10.005
[11] M. S. Walid and N. V. Zaytseva, “Upper Cervical Spine
Injuries in Elderly Patients,” Australian Family Physician,
Vol. 38, No. 1-2, 2009, pp. 43-45.
Copyright © 2011 SciRes. OJMN