 Psychology 2013. Vol.4, No.9, 717-728 Published Online September 2013 in SciRes (http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.49102 Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 717 Tolerance of Ambiguity: A Review of the Recent Literature Adrian Furnham1,2, Joseph Marks3 1Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK 2Norwegian Business School, Olso, Norway 3School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK Email: a.furnham@ucl.ac.uk Received July 10th, 2013; revised August 8th, 2013; accepted August 31st, 2013 Copyright © 2013 Adrian Furnham, Joseph Marks. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. This review paper attempts to update the literature on tolerance of ambiguity (TA) and related concepts since a previous review (Furnham & Ribchester, 1995). Various related concepts like Uncertainly Avoid- ance and In/Tolerance of Uncertainly are reviewed. Both correlational and experimental studies of TA are reviewed and tabulated. Further, an attempt was made to identify and critique various different question- naires design to measure TA. Recommendations for the use of these tests in research are made. The rea- sons for progress and lack of progress in this field are highlighted. Keywords: Tolerance; Ambiguity; Review Introduction The concept of tolerance of ambiguity (TA), which was originally developed by Frenkel-Brunswik (1948), has attracted a great deal of research over the last 60 years (Merrotsy, 2013). Her paper, that related TA to authoritarianism, has since been cited nearly 10,000 times (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levin- son, & Sanford, 1950). TA has generally been conceived as a personality variable or individual difference factor (Budner, 1962) and has been used in a variety of applied fields, including clinical psychology (Lachance et al., 1999), medicine (Geller et al., 1993) and organisational behaviour (Judge et al. 1999). This paper attempts to update review by Furnham (1994) and Furn- ham and Ribchester (1995) on the conceptions, correlates and measurement of TA. History of the Concept Frenkel-Brunswick (1949) defined TA as an “emotional and perceptual personality variable”. She was influenced by the work of Jaensch (1938) whose work was to influence many others (Eysenck, 1954). She concluded: “In the present paper, an attempt was made to discuss denial of emotional ambiva- lence and intolerance of cognitive ambiguity as but different aspects of what may be a fairly coherent characteristic. An underlying emotional conflict between glorification and hostil- ity in the attitude towards parents, sex and one’s own social identity previously demonstrated in children inclined toward rigid social dichotomizing as revealed by ethnic prejudice is taken as the impetus for experiments in memory, perception, and related topics, devised to test tolerance of ambiguity on an emotionally more neutral ground. There is some indication of a prevalence of premature reduction of ambiguous cognitive patterns to certainty in the prejudiced subjects, as revealed by a clinging to the familiar, or by a superimposition of one or many distorting cliches upon stimuli which are more manageable in a more simple and stereotyped fashion. There is some indication that in the case of distinct intolerance of emotional ambivalence one may as a rule be able to locate at least some aspects of in- tolerance of cognitive ambiguity although these may often by more apparent on a higher level than that of perception paper.” (p. 140). Frenkel-Brunswick (1951) set out many behavioural features of TA including resistance to reversal of apparent fluctuating stimuli; the early selection and maintenance of one solution in a perceptually ambiguous situation; inability to allow for the possibility of good and bad traits in the same person; accep- tance of attitude statements representing a rigid; black-white view of life; seeking for certainty; a rigid dichotomising into fixed categories; premature closure, and remaining closed ex- cept to familiar characteristics of stimuli. Thus TA was con- ceived as a salient, multi-faceted predictive variable in a variety of behavioural settings. Frenkel-Brunswick’s (1949, 1951) definition of the concept was generated by case study material gleaned from interviews of persons high or low on this construct. She argued that TA generalises to the various aspects of emotional and cognitive functioning of the individual, characterising cognitive style, belief and attitude systems, interpersonal and social functioning and problem solving behaviour. She also related TA to other personality variables, predicting a positive relationship with the authoritarian family of personality traits. Since then the topic has attracted considerable research and remains a well-used variable to this day (Anderson & Schwartz, 1992; Merrotsy, 2013). Early Studies Many of the early studies in this area were psychometric studies that attempted to construct a valid, self-report, measure
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS of TA. Budner (1962) defined TA as “the tendency to perceive ambiguous situations as desirable” and set about one of the first measures in the field. Budner’s (1962) paper has been cited over 1000 times. McLain (1993) included contextual informa- tion, defining TA as “a range, from rejection to attraction, of reactions to stimuli perceived as unfamiliar, complex, dynami- cally uncertain or subject to multiple conflicting interpreta- tions” (p. 184). There has expectedly been debate on the dimensionality of the TA concept. Durrheim and Foster (1997) propose that TA is a context-specific construct, not a personality trait, and others advocate the use of contextualised measures (Herman, Stevens, Bird, Mendenhall, & Oddou, 2010). TA is however usually measured on a one-dimensional scale: those who are intolerant of ambiguity are described as having a tendency to resort to black-and-white solutions, and character- ised by rapid and overconfident judgement, often at the neglect of reality (Frenkel-Brunswik, 1949). At the other end of the scale, ambiguous situations are perceived as desirable, chal- lenging and interesting, usually by individuals who score highly on an Openness to Experience scale (Caligiuri, Jacobs, & Farr, 2000) and show both sensation-seeking and risk-taking behav- iour (McLain, 1993; Lauriola, Levin, & Hart, 2007; McLain, 2009). Generally for those with low TA there is an aversive reaction to ambiguous situations because the lack of information makes it difficult to assess risk and correctly make a decision. These situations are perceived as a threat and source of discomfort. Reactions to the perceived threat are stress, avoidance, delay, suppression, or denial (Budner, 1962; MacDonald, 1970; McLain, 1993; Furnham & Ribchester, 1995). In more recent work researchers have altered their focus to- wards examining how TA influences the perception of situa- tions and decision making (Yurtsever, 2001, 2008; Van Hook & Steele, 2002; McLain, 2009). Instead it is thought that con- structs that are defined by an absence of information (e.g. risk-taking behaviour) are more relevant and are more useful validating TA measures. The TA research literature appears to have three different features: there have been work on similar concepts to TA which is discussed below; there have been an increase of experimental over correlational studies; and a more of an interest in the ef- fects of TA in the work environment. Conceptual and Definitional Issues Other similar concepts have been described which are clearly very similar to TA like Uncertainty Avoidance and Risk-Taking Propensity. Hofstede (1984) recognised uncertainty avoidance as a characteristic of cultures and developed an uncertainty avoidance index. The distribution of the personality variable “uncertainty avoidance” has been studied in different societies, thus making it a “sociological variable”, where uncertainty avoidance was defined as “the extent to which people feel threatened by ambiguous situations, and have created beliefs and institutions that try to avoid these” (p. 419). Most research- ers interested in uncertainty avoidance are cross-cultural or organisational psychologists who are interested in comparing groups of individuals, rather than seeing it as an individual preference or trait. As a consequence, these researchers seem not to have developed many self-report measures of uncertainty avoidance. There is also a clinical literature on Tolerance of Uncertainty (TU) which has been conceived of as a cognitive disposition that confers risk of Generalised Anxiety Disorder (Birrell, Meares, Wilkinson & Freeston, 2011). Various self-report mea- sures have been developed which are often validated against measures of anxiety, depression and worry (Carleton, Norton & Asmundson, 2007). TU is associated with worries and negative expectations of the future and is therefore often involved in research of anxiety disorders (Ladouceur et al., 2000). TU is usually measured using the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (Freeston et al., 1994), which is made up of 27 items. Its inter- nal consistency is high, α = 0.91 and Dugas et al. (1997) re- ported a test-retest reliability of 0.78 over a five week period. The scale is used as a clinical tool in the diagnosis of GAD (Freeston et al., 1994). It continues to be examined for its psy- chometric properties (Buhr & Dugas, 2002; Fergus & Wu, 2012). Green and Roger (2001) argued that there is a clear relation- ship between TA and TU but that the former is used primarily in cognitive studies on decision-making, memory and percep- tion “all of which are oriented towards cognitive processes rather than stress and emotion” (p. 521). They developed a three factor scale two factors of which (emotional uncertainty and cognitive uncertainty) were modestly correlated (respec- tively: r = .18 and r = .37, N = 204). In their review of the fac- tor analytic studies of the best known scale in the area, Birrell et al (2011) found evidence of two factors: Desire for Predict- ability and Active Engagement in Seeking Certainty; and Pa- ralysis of Cognition and Cognition in the Face of Uncertainty. The TA, TU and uncertainty avoidance concepts have been used interchangeably (Stewart, Carland, Carland, Watson, & Sweo, 2003; McLain, 1993; Majid & Pragasam, 1997; Grenier, Barrette, & Ladouceur, 2005) but efforts have been made to show that the concepts are not identical. Ellsberg (1961) de- fined ambiguity as a lack of information that is necessary to understand a situation or to identify all of the possible out- comes. Krohne (1989, 1993) concluded that whilst ambiguity is a property of the stimulus, uncertainty is the emotional state that is provoked by it. Grenier et al. (2005) argued that the main difference between TA and TU is the time frame referred to. TA describes a trait that focuses on an individual’s reaction to an ambiguous situation in the present. TU, on the other hand, describes a future-orientated trait, where the individual is re- acting to the uncertainty of the future. This discrimination partly explains why the TA and TU literature tend to have sep- arate areas of focus. TA is used in the cognitive and experi- mental literature and TU in the clinical literature. Risk-taking propensity is also very similar to TA (McLain, 2009). Lauriola, Levin and Hart (2007) argue that there is a stable dispositional trait that underlies risky decision making and decision making under ambiguity in experimental tasks. Ellsberg (1961) distinguished decisions under ambiguity from risky decision making in terms of knowledge of outcomes and probabilities: Behavioural decision scientists usually define ambiguous decision making as a situation in which there is an unknown distribution of outcome probabilities for at least one of the options. Whereas the probabilities are known in risky decision-making, but outcomes are not. However it remains true that despite work on these subtly different and related concepts there is still no very clear opera- tional definition of TA at the facet level or a clear differentia- tion between the manifestations and correlates of TA. Nor has Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 718
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS there been any strong theoretical development in the area. Both may account for the steady, but uninspiring, developments in the area. Correlational Studies There have been many attempts to look at the concurrent, convergent and discriminant validity of TA with studies corre- lating scores with other established measures. Most of the work in this area remains correlational. The relationships between TA and other personality variables have been supported by correlations in self-report question- naires and has been used to validate scales of TA. Budner’s (1962) 16-item scale positively correlated with authoritarianism and this was used as evidence of construct validity. Mac- Donald’s (1970) AT-20 correlated with Rokeach’s dogmatism scale and the Walk’s A Scale correlated with ethnocentrism (O’Connor, 1952). Correlations also exist with other forms of measurement of TA. Million (1957) measure TA by the autokinetic phenome- non and found a relationship with authoritarianism. However despite some evidence for this relationship, research has not always proved conclusive. For example, Feather (1969) found that the Budner scale’s measurement of TA did not relate to dogmatism. We set about an extensive search for all TA and TU papers published since 1995. Many simply mentioned the concepts, and we decided to review only those which had actually used a measure of TA or TU in the research. We then decided to tabu- late the results showing the range of papers published, what measures they used and what they found. These are shown in Table 1 where 30 studies are reviewed. Because details of the studies are provided in the table the results will not be considered in detail. Rather, four observa- tions from this research effort can be summarised. First, they use a wide range of measures of TA, not all of which correlate very highly with each other. Second, many have modest popu- lation groups, though a number have populations over 200. Third, the number of variables correlated with measures of TA were extremely varied from art preference, though identity conflict to thinking style. There seemed no thematic or pro- grammatic effort on any research group in this area. Correla- tions tended to be modest. Fourth, most studies had their hy- potheses confirmed showing how TA was conceptually related to a variety of other measures and behaviours. Experimental Work There have also been one or two experimental studies in this area. However they have been the exception rather than the rule. For instance Lauriola and Levin (2001) designed an ecologi- cally valid experiment that compared attitudes towards ambigu- ity and risk. They showed that differences in attitude towards ambiguity are consistent with attitudes towards risk, in that a preference for the ambiguous predicts a preference for a risky options. However on further inspection, the relationship only proved significant when participants were avoiding a loss in the Risky Decision-making Task as opposed to seeking a gain. It was concluded that the extra processing in the “loss” condition meant that attitudes towards ambiguity were more important in the decision-making procedure. Lauriola, Levin and Hart (2007) repeated this experiment and found that the Ambiguity-Probability Tradeoff task negatively correlated with a TA self-report questionnaire (MSTAT-I) (r = −.15; p < 0.05) as well as optimism scores on the Life Orienta- tion Test-Revised, and positively correlated with regret-based decision style. A high score on this task predicted subsequent risky choices in a follow up study a month later. It was also predictive of later ambiguous choices in a different domain. These findings support the existence of a stable dispositional trait underlying reactions to risk and ambiguity. The Measurement of TA Given that the TA construct has been around for so long, it is no surprise that a number of measures exist (see Table 2). To date we have found 8 self-report measures. However we ac- knowledge other, unpublished, tests are recorded (Saunders, 1955) or those where little psychometric work was attempted (Eysenck, 1954). Eysenck’s early measure was a simple 14- item true-false test but appeared to have good concurrent valid- ity. He reported on a non-verbal pictorial intolerance of ambi- guity test which was 8 drawings of a dog turning slowly and by degrees into a cat. The predicted and confirmed hypothesis was that rigid people would continue to cling to the original “dog” concept long after it had turned into a cat. Most of the tests of TA are however self-report question- naires. One of the best known and well used scales in this area was developed 30 years ago by Budner (1962) who devised a 16-item (half positive, half negative) scale which was described in great detail. He argued that each item had to tap at least one postulated indicator of perceived threat, namely phenomenol- ogical submissions or denial, operative submission or denial. Items referred to one of either of three types of ambiguous situations: novelty, complexity and insolubility. The scale was validated on 17 different, mainly student, populations and shown to be free of acquiescent and social desirability response tendencies. Although the test correlation was good (0.85 over 2 months) the internal alpha was poor (0.49). Various forms of validity were examined including concurrent and construct. Budner’s scale was correlated with rankings of individuals on the basis of short biographies, peer ratings, and measures of conventionality, belief in divine power, attendence at religious services, dogmatism about religious beliefs and attitudes to censorship. The total scale also correlated positively with au- thoritarianism, idealism of and submission to parents, Machia- vellianism, career choice in medical students etc. Not all the correlations were significant and most were in the 0.20 to 0.40 range but they were sufficiently consistent to suggest that the measure had content, concurrent and construct validity. The wording of items in this scale have been criticised for their failure to represent the appropriate stimulus, or even suggest ambiguity at all (McLain, 2009). The items are also argued to be confounded by reference to specific situation, which may elicit misleading reactions. Budner saw TA as a “non-specific” trait that does not lead to specific behaviours or evaluations that are not manifestations of TA itself. The Budner scale has perhaps attracted most attention and is used most frequently in TA research. Rydell and Rosen (1966) and Rydell (1966) reported on the development and validation of another scale. The scale consisted of 16 true-false items which had been constructed on a “a-priori basis” (Rydell & Rosen, 1966: p. 151) with relatively limited validation. Test- etest reliabilities over a month with 41 students yielded an r = r Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 719
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 720 Table 1. Table showing TA measures. Authors N TA measure Outcome measure r/p values Findings Thalbourne et al. (2000) 100 AT-20 Transliminality r = −0.02 Transliminality did not correlate with TA. Litman, (2010) 372 AT-20 Dispositional interest (I)-type curiosity; Deprivation (D)-type curiosity; Anxiety; Anger; Depression r = 0.36; −0.15; −0.02; 0.03; −0.15 AT-20 scale was positively correlated with I-type curiosity and negatively correlated to D-type curiosity and anger. Weisbrod, (2009) 157 AT-20 Ethical decision making p = 0.018, < 0.01 Low TA predicts less willingness to violate ethical norms in both personal and organizational settings. High TA subjects were more likely to violate ethical norms whilst experiencing high negative affect. Hazen, Overstreet, Jones-Farmer, Field, (2012) 340 Multiple Stimulus Types Ambiguity Tolerance Scale-II (MSTAT-II) Willingness to pay for remanufactured goods p < 0.001; p < 0.001 Consumers’ TA positively correlated with their willingness to pay for remanufactured products; TA positively correlated with perceived quality of remanufactured products. Bardi, Guerra, Sharadeh, & Ramdeny, (2009) 510 Uncertainty Tolerance Scale (UTS; Dalbert, 1999) Openness r = −0.42, p< 0.01; r = −0.25, p < 0.01 Intolerance of ambiguity negatively correlated with openness. Bardi, Guerra, Sharadeh, & Ramdeny, (2009) 510 Uncertainty Tolerance Scale (UTS; Dalbert, 1999) Challenge appraisal r = −0.12, p < 0.05Intolerance of ambiguity negatively correlated with challenge appraisal. Life satisfaction r = −0.14, p< 0.05; r = −0.15, p < 0.05 Intolerance of ambiguity negatively correlated with life satisfaction. Positive affect r = −0.24, p< 0.01; r = −0.18, p <0.01 Intolerance of ambiguity negatively correlated with positive affect. Threat appraisal r = 0.30, p < 0.01Intolerance of ambiguity positively correlated with threat appraisal. Negative affect r = 0.33, p < 0.01Intolerance of ambiguity positively correlated with negative affect. Anxiety r = 0.38, p < 0.01Intolerance of ambiguity positively correlated with anxiety. Teoh & Foo, (1997) 70 AT-20 Entrepreneurial performancep < 0.07 TA moderates the relationship between role conflict and performance measures. Teoh & Foo, (1997) 70 AT-21 Entrepreneurial performancep < 0.01 High TA correlates with better performance. Lal & Hassel, (1998) 64 Budner’s TIA Perceived usefulness of information characteristics of management accounting systems (MAS) p < 0.05 Managers with high TA perceive non-conventional MAS as more useful when perceived environmental uncertainty is high than those with low TA. Perceived usefulness of information in accounting systems (MAS) p < 0.01 TA has a stronger effect on MAS in large firms than small firms. Firoozabadi, & Bahredar, (2006) 240 Budner’s TIA Medical students’ demographics p < 0.05 Men scored lower than women on the TA scale. Medical speciality preferencen/a There was no difference in TA level between medical speciality preferences. Tapanes, Smith, & White, (2009) 66 Hofstede’s Value Survey Perceived effect of dissonance in online learning p = 0.002; p = 0.015 Learners from low TA cultures felt it was important for their instructors to take into act their cultural background and that they be informed about differences between their culture and that of the course. Perceived effect of dissonance in online learning p = 0.007 High TA cultures reported being more motivated to learn whilst low TA cultures were intimated. Perceived effect of dissonance in online learning p = 0.047 High TA cultures had higher participation rates (controlling for language). Perceived effect of dissonance in online learning p = 0.168; p= 0.05; p = 0.216; p = 0.212 No significant differences regarding their instructor’s awareness, consideration culture, silenced experiences and feelings of alienation for the TA dimension.
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Continued Chong, (1998) 63 AT-20 Managerial perforance (via management accounting systems (MAS) information) TA and MAS, r = 0.381 MAS X TA: p = 0.012 TA is negatively correlated with MAS, which has a direct effect on managerial perfomance. Hartmann & Slapnicar, (2012) 178 AT-20 Justice perceptions with the use of outcome measures r = 0.318, p < 0.001Managers with low TA judge an evaluation process more fairly. Swami, Stieger, Pietschnig & Voracek, (2010) 240 MAT-50 Preference for surrealist art r = −0.22, p< 0.05; r = −0.22, p < 0.05; r = −0.25, p < 0.001 TA positively correlates with a liking for surrealist art (TA subscales: Philosophy, Problem-solving, Art Forms). Preference for surrealist filmsr = −0.19, p< 0.05; r = −0.18, p < 0.05 TA positively correlates with a liking for surrealist films (TA subscales: Problem-solving, Art Forms). Rong & Grover, (2009) 126 MAT-50 Technological knowledge renewal effectiveness (t = 2.32, p = 0.01)TA has a positive impact on technological knowledge renewal effectiveness. Iyer, McBride, & Reckers, (2012) 78 AT-20 Capital investment proposal recommendation (with/without a decision aid) F = 5.09, p = 0.027Low TA Ss applied decision aids when making a decision on an ambiguous investment. Buhr & Dugas, (2006) 197 Budner’s TIA Intolerance of uncertainty r = 0.42, p <0.001TA has a positive correlation with tolerance of uncertainty. Worry r = 0.27, p < 0.001TA has a negative relationship with worry. Self-oriented perfectionismr = 0.19, p < 0.01TA has a negative relationship with self-oriented perfectionism. Socially-prescribed perfectionism r = 0.35, p < 0.001TA has a negative relationship with socially-prescribed perfectionism. Other-oriented perfectionismr = 0.15, p < 0.05TA has a negative relationship with other-oriented perfectionism. Perceived mastery r = -0.14, p < 0.05TA has a positive relationship with perceived mastery. Perceived constraints r = 0.32, p < 0.001TA has a negative relationship with perceived constraint. Age r = −0.24, p < 0.01TA increases with age. Wolfradt, Oubaid, Straube, Bischoff & Mischo, (1999) 374 The Scale of Interpersonal Intolerance of Ambiguity (SIA) by Wolfradt and Rademacher Schizotypal personality disorder r = 0.27, p < 0.001; r = 0.25, p < 0.001; r = 0.43, p < 0.001 TA has a negative relationship with Schizotypal personality disorder (cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganizational deficits). Information processing (need for cognition) r = -0.19, p < 0.001TA has a positive relationship with this type of information processing (need for cognition). General Self-Efficacy r = −0.31, p < 0.001TA has a positive relationship with general self-efficacy. Furnham & Avison, (1997) 62 AT-20 Painting preferences r = 0.31, p < 0.05TA is significantly related positively to surrealist (fewer elements) paintings. Ironside, Jefferies & Martin, (2009) n/a The Multiple Stimulus Types Ambiguity Tolerance Scale-I (MSTAT-I) Achievement of patient safety competencies p > 0.05 TA did not correlate with nurses’ patients safety competencies. Leong & Ward, (2000) 106 MSTAT-I Identity conflict r = −0.32, p < 0.005High TA is a significant predictor of identity in Chinese sojourners in Singapore. Le, Haller, Langer, Cou voisie , 2012 75 Budner’s TIA Mindfulness r = −0.35, p < 0.01TA positively correlated with mindfulness. Thinking style r = −0.54, p< 0.01; r = −0.43, p < 0.01 TA positively correlated with thinking style (mean; concrete). Affect r = −0.01; r = 0.13TA did not correlate with pre-experimental (positive or negative) affect. Westerberg, Singh & Häckner, (1997) 139 Modified from Lorsch and Morse Firms’ financial performanceb = 0.26, p < 0.01CEOs with high TA were related to firms with high financial performance. Firms’ market performanceb = 0.26, p < 0.01CEOs with high TA were related to firms with high market performance. Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 721
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Continued Firms’ market orientation p > 0.05 CEOs’ TA did not correlate with firms that emphasize planning orientation. Firms’ planning orientationp > 0.05 CEOs’ TA did not correlate with firms that emphasize market orientation. Richardson, Jain & Dick, (1996) 582 Budner’s TIA Private brand proneness p > 0.05 TA did not correlate with private brand proneness. Value for money perceptions o private label brands. β = −0.117, p < 0.05 TA positively correlates with money perceptions (value for money). Reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment. β = 0.251, p < 0.05TA had a negative relationship with reliance on extrinsic cues in quality assessment. Altinay, Madanoglu, Daniele & Lashley, (2012) 205 Acedo and Jones scale: modified from Lorsch and Morse Intention to start a businessr = 0.274, p > 0.05There was no relationship between TA and intention to start a business. Risk-taking propensity r = 0.318, p = 0.426There was a positive relationship between tolerance of ambiguity and risk taking propensity. Caligiuri, Tarique, (2012) 641 Modified Gupta and Govindarajan (1984) Non-work cross-cultural experiences r = 0.28, p < 0.01TA correlated positively with non-work cross-cultural experiences. Organization-initiated cross-cultural experiences r = 0.06, p < 0.01TA correlated positively with “organization- initiated” cross cultural experiences. Dynamic cross-cultural competencies R2 = 0.26, p < 0.01TA correlated positively with dynamic cross-cultural competencies. Neuroticism r = 0.07, p > 0.05TA did not correlate with neuroticism. Extraversion r = 0.37, p < 0.01TA had a strong, positive correlation with extraversion. Agreeableness r = −0.19, p < 0.05TA correlated negatively with agreeableness. Openness r = 0.29, p < 0.01TA had a strong, positive correlation with openness. Conscientiousness r = 0.00, p > 0.05TA did not correlate with conscientiousness. Neill &Rose, (2007) 167 Modified MSTAT-I Equivocality t = 0.27, p < 0.01An organisations TA correlates with equivocality. Market-focused strategic flexibility t = 0.21, p < 0.01An organizations TA is positively correlated with market-focused strategic flexibility. Rajagopal & Hamouz, (2009) 111 Budner’s TIA Willingness to try (A factor of the Food Attitude Behavior Openness Scale (FABOS)) r = 0.332, p < 0.01TA positively correlated with the factor “willingness to try”. Seeking novelty (FABOS) r = 0.447, p < 0.01TA positively correlated with the factor “seeking novelty”. Enjoy novelty (FABOS) r = 0.212, p > 0.01TA did not correlate with the “enjoy novelty” factor. Conditional openess (FABOS)r = 0.098, p > 0.01TA did not correlate with the “conditional openness” factor. Internationalization decisionsp < 0.05 CEOs’ international orientation did not relate to TA. Risk associated with internationalization p < 0.001 CEOs with high TA perceived lower levels of risk. Proactive disposition to internationalization p < 0.05 TA did not correlate with a proactive disposition to internationalization. Carter & Hall, (2008) 279 Budner’s TIA Observational Test of Ecological Sensitivity p < 0.05 TA and openness to experience gave a combined score to assess cognitive openess, which correlated significantly positively with the Observational Test of Ecological Sensitivity. Gurel, Altinay, & Daniele, (2010) 206 Acedo and Jones (2007) = modified Lorsch and Morse (1974) Entrepreneurial intention p > 0.05 TA was not associated with intentions to start a business. Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 722
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 723 Table 2. Table showing measures of the TA scales. Author Name of scale N No. of items Dimensions Herman, Stevens, Bird, Mendenhall, Oddou, (2010) The tolerance of ambiguity scale 2351 12 (1) 4 McLain (2009) Multiple stimulus types ambiguity tolerance scale-II (MSTAT-II) 870 13 (1) 3 Buhr & Dugas (2002) Intolerance of ambiguity scale 276 27 4 Lange & Houran (1999) Rasch model AT-20 110 18 1 Durrheim & Foster (1996) Attitudinal ambiguity tolerance scale 421 45 4 McLain (1993) Multiple stimulus types ambiguity tolerance scale-I 148 22 1 Norton (1975) MAT 50 1496 61 8 MacDonald (1970) AT-20 789 20 1 Budner (1962) 16 item scale 947 16 1 O’Connor (1952) Walk Unpublished8 1 0.71 and with 105 students over 2 months r = 0.57 but there was no evidence of the test’s internal reliability. The test was in part validated with the use of semantic differential ratings of contradictory and non-contradictory adjective-noun concept combinations (Rydell, 1966). MacDonald (1970) however, attempted some psychometric evaluation of the Rydell-Rosen scale but added 4 extra items. This larger scale had a test-retest reliability of 0.63 over 6 months and was cross-validated on nearly 800 undergraduates. The test was correlated with Rokeach’s dogmatism scale, the Gough-Sanford Rigidity scale and church attendence but not social desirability. The split-half reliability was also satisfactory at 0.73, and MacDonald noted that it“shows promise of being a useful instrument for the measurement and further investigation of ambiguity tolerance” (p. 797). Lange and Houran (1999) praised the AT-20 scale for its convergent validity and internal consistency, but argued that an Item Response Theory (ITR) framework have more appropriate scaling properties for use in structural modeling. In particular, they propose the Rasch (1960) model because “Rasch scaling requires no iterative estimation procedures” (p.468). The Rasch model AT-20 (Lange & Houran, 1999) only differentiates itself from the AT-20 from a scaling point of view—there are no additional items but two were removed. It provides no new information for evidence of validity. Results showed that the positive item-rest point biserial correlations provide evidence of the scale’s uni-dimensionality, correlations were consistent with the local independence assumption, the discrimination parameter values show the data fits with the Rasch model and the person fit to the Rasch model was satis- factory. The sample size was large enough to yield an adequate item separation value (3.72), which indicates the estimated item locations have a KR-20 reliability index of 0.93. The internal consistency was 0.68 (KR-20), which is slightly lower than the value reported by MacDonald (1970) but still just about satis- factory considering the number of items. The Rasch approach is said to differ to the classical test theory with respect to estimat- ing tolerance of ambiguity and assessing the error of measure- ment associated with such estimates. In the mid-1970s Norton (1975) argued that the extant pa- per-and-pencil measures of TA were “flawed by low internal reliability and the absence of adequate validity evidence” (p. 607). This he believed was in part due to ambiguities associated with the term ambiguous which was used in 8 different ways to describe: multiple meanings; vagueness, incompleteness, frag- mentation; as a probability; unstructured; lack of information; uncertainty; inconsistencies, contradiction, contraries and un- clearness. He therefore developed a 50-item “measure” (MAT- 50) which was tested seven times to develop high reliability which was r = 0.38 (Kuder-Richardson) and with a test-retest reliability of 0.86 after 10 - 12 weeks. He also attempted to determine 3 types of validity: content validity (through content analysis and faking studies), criterion-related (through correla- tions with measures of dogmatism and rigidity) and construct validity (through measures of willingness to volunteer for an ambiguous study; aesthetic judgement; a content analysis of verbal behaviour and behavioural dramatisation). As predicted, high TA Ss tended to volunteer more for undefined experiments, to use different aesthetic judgements and be more dramatic in problem-solving groups. Norton (1975) ended his paper suggesting seven research questions the scale may be used to investigate most of which had been tested before but not when using a self-report TA measure: what is the cognitive process during an ambiguous situation; how can the information theorist account for ambigu- ity; is it possible to use the measure of TA to help identify therapeutic problems; do groups prefer leaders who are TA; how is trusting behaviour related to TA: to what degree is TA culture bound; what is the function of TA in a conflict resolu- tion situation. Nearly 40 years ago Lorsch and Morse (1974) argued that managers who often face ambiguous decisions have to be more willing to take risks. They developed a 7-item scale with the aim to test members of organizations, specifically managers’ TA. Therefore this scale has been prominent mainly in business journals and has been modified a number of times in order to fit more appropriately with researchers’ needs (Westerberg, Singh & Häckner, 1997; Acedo & Jones, 2007; Gurel, Altinay & Daniele, 2010; Caligiuri & Tarique, 2012). Among others, Gupta and Govindarajan (1984) reduced the number of items to
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS 4, and reported an internal reliability of 0.57. Motivated by the psychometric weakness of widely used measures of TA, McLain (1993) developed a new 22-item measure called the Multiple Stimulus Types Ambiguity Toler- ance (MSTAT). McLain attempted to redefine TA so that the three conceptual perspectives of TA could be separately defined and integrated. These three concepts are: TA as a source of threat from novel, complex and insoluble stimuli (Budner, 1962), ambiguity as term for second order probability (Ellsberg, 1961), and TA as a link to the authoritarianism family (Fren- kel-Brunswik, 1949). McLain defined TA as “a range, from rejection to attraction, of reactions to stimuli perceived as un- familiar, complex, dynamically uncertain, or subject to multiple conflicting interpretations” (p. 184). A factor analysis of 148 respondents supported a uni-dimensional model, a general tol- erance for ambiguity. The scale was found to have good inter- nal consistency, α = 0.86. Evidence of the scale’s concurrent validity for the scale comes from significant positive correla- tions with other TA scales (Budner’s (1962) 16-item scale, Storey and Aldag’s (1983) 8-item scale and MacDonald’s (1970) 20-item scale as well as significantly correlating with willingness to take risks, receptivity to change and a negative correlation with dogmatism. The adequate psychometric prop- erties and refined construct of this scale make it one of the more popular measures in recent times. In 2009, McLain refined the MSTAT scale. The MSTAT-II is a 13-item scale derived from the original 22 items. The re- duced number of items means that respondents use less cogni- tive resources completing the questionnaire. Items were re- moved from the MSTAT-I on the basis of feedback from re- searchers and respondents who used the questionnaire. The remaining items were kept if they added to the overall construct, correlated with the scale, and did not confound it through con- text-specificity or incomprehensibility. The data was collected from a sample of university students and firefighter-emergency medical technicians (n = 870). The internal consistency reli- ability was 0.83, which is good despite being slightly lower than the MSTAT-I. Three factors were identified by a factor analysis, however a scree plot showed a distinct first factor only. This factor corresponded to ambiguity tolerance in general and confirmatory factor analysis suggested the one-dimensional theoretical model is appropriate. McLain found that MSTAT-II correlated significantly and positively with MacDonald’s AT-20, sensation seeking, per- ceived risk, perceived uncertainty, which provides evidence for concurrent validity. The scale correlated negatively with so- matic tension and social desirability. The correlation with Bud- ner’s scale however, was not significant. McLain argued that this finding may be due to the Budner scale’s low reliability and poor item wording. He later found that the Budner scores had a multidimensional structure and therefore should not be seen to undermine the strength of the MSTAT-II. Despite these arguments, it should be noted that the MSTAT-I did signifi- cantly correlate with the Budner scale and it may be possible that because items have been removed, the MSTAT-II is less comprehensive. Therefore this scale is recommended over the MSTAT-I when space is limited or when participants could potentially become cognitively overloaded. Durrheim and Foster (1997) did not conceive TA as psycho- logical trait, but as a content specific construct. This is consis- tent with Frenkel-Brunswick’s (1949) original construct of TA as an “attitudinal variable”, which was not assumed to general- ise across different social objects. Among others, Durrheim and Foster (1997) proposed that intercorrelations of TA measures are “spurious relationships between shared attitudinal scale content” (p. 741) and that the relationship is a methodological artefact. This accounts for the failure to correlate different ex- perimental procedures (e.g. Million, 1957). They argue that factor analysis (e.g. Furnham, 1994) show TA to be multidi- mensional and conclude that there is little evidence for regard- ing TA as a stable and generalised personality trait. Durrheim and Foster (1997), who are social, not personality, psychologists, developed the Attitudinal Ambiguity Tolerance scale (AAT) in response to these objections, which is based on the uni-polar scaling procedure that was originated by Kaplan (1972) and subsequently used by Scott and colleagues to assess ambivalence (Scott, 1966, 1969; Scott, Osgood, & Peterson, 1979). The scale uses a wide range of authority figures as scale items and participants are asked to express their degree of am- bivalence. This scale measures evaluative performance and is therefore thought to model Frenkel-Brunswick’s original de- scription of prejudiced and non-prejudiced children. The scale was found to have an adequate internal reliability (α = 0.81) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.66). A factor analysis revealed four factors, which suggests a multi-dimensional structure and supports the original hypothesis. Evidence for concurrent valid- ity for this scale comes from a significant positive correlation with the Ambivalence scale that the AAT was based on. The AAT scale was not significantly correlated with Bud- ner’s scale, however this is inconclusive because not only is the Budner scale thought to lack reliability and validity, but there are theoretical underlying differences between the two scales. Criterion groups were also used to validate the AAT scale be- cause of the political context in South Africa at the time of testing. Although Durrheim and Foster’s criticisms of the TA concept seem valid, they have largely been ignored by con- temporary research for three reasons. Firstly, the focus of the TA concept has shifted away from prejudice and authoritarian- ism and moved towards reactions in response to insufficient information. Secondly, the psychometric qualities of TA scales have increased. Third, empirical research supports a one-di- mensional theoretical model (McLain, 2009; Benjamin et al. 1996; Furnham & Ribchester, 1995), which describes TA as “unitary yet multifaceted” (Herman et al., 2010: p. 59). It should be noted that Wolfradt and Rademacher (1999) de- veloped and validated a scale for interpersonal TA. This scale was designed for use as a clinical tool. The scale is not widely used however, despite good internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86; Wolfradt, Oubaid, Straube, Bischoff, & Mischo, 1999). Herman et al. (2010) proposed a new measure of TA that aimed to better understand its link to cross-cultural phenomena, improved conceptual dimensionality and psychometric evi- dence. They attributed the disagreement in construct dimen- sionality to the diversity of research contexts, arguing that overly general items may not be suitable for all the diverse concepts of TA. The MSTAT-II may suffer from over-gener- alisation, although the author admitted this himself (McLain, 2009). Instead, Herman et al. (2010) reasoned that context-depend- ent measures should be developed in areas that may have prob- lems if they use the generalised conception. Their measure focuses on cross-cultural contexts. They used Budner’s (1962) conceptualisation and measure of TA (because it has been so Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 724
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS influential despite its flaws) as a basis on which they performed an exploratory factor analysis, assessment of internal consis- tency and item-total correlations, then added and removed items to improve the measure (n = 2351). The new items were designed to fit with prior conceptions of TA (Budner, 1962; McLain, 1993) and relevant to a cross-cultural context. They found the overall internal consistency to be acceptable (α = 0.73). Factor analysis distinguished 4 factors, but the measure was found to fit a one-dimensional theoretical framework be- cause the internal consistencies of the individual dimensions were not high enough to support multidimensionality. The four factors were; valuing diverse others, change, challenging per- spectives and unfamiliarity. Valuing diverse others has not appeared in other recent conceptualisation (McLain, 1993, 2009; Furnham, 1994), which the authors relate to the interpersonal nature of cross-cultural settings. This 12-item scale is a useful tool for measuring TA in cross-cultural contexts and it may revolutionise the measurement of TA, starting a trend in the development of context-specific measures. It should be noted that Wolfradt and Rademacher (1999) de- veloped and validated a scale for interpersonal TA. This scale was designed for use as a clinical tool. The scale is not widely used however, despite good internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86; Wolfradt, Oubaid, Straube, Bischoff, & Mischo, 1999). Conclusion The TA concept has gone through changes since its concep- tion in 1948, when it was synonymous with authoritarianism and prejudice. The focus has now shifted to reflect the contem- porary definition of ambiguity (Ellsberg, 1962). Researchers have ducked the questions about where TA sits in Big Five Factor space. Is TA a second or third order facet or does it be- long outside the big five a little like Locus of Control or other “cognitive personality variables”? The papers in this area still lack sophistication. For instance there appear to be no studies that have attempted to determine the heritability of TA. This would help differentiate between the social psychological conception of TA as a set of attitudes vs the differential psychology conception of a stable, perhaps even biologically based trait. Nor have the tests been frequently subjected to structural equation modelling to determine both the facets or factors of TA as well as its determinants. However, the interest among clinicians in the TU concept may suggest that it is closely linked positively to Neuroticism and negatively to Openness-to-Experience. The interest in TA seems to have shifted from differential and social psychologist to clinical and organisational psycholo- gists who see it as measure of adaptation and healthy function- ing. There yet remains a need to do some psychometric house- keeping as done by Furnham (1994) to look at the relationship between the existent measures, but perhaps more importantly to integrate TA measures and theory to modern psychometric methods as well as cognitive neuro-science. REFERENCES Acedo, F. J., & Jones, M. V. (2007). Speed of internationalisation and entrepreneurial cognition. Journal of World Business, 42, 236-252. doi:10.1016/j.jwb.2007.04.012 Altinay, L., Madanoglu, M., Daniele, R., & Lashley, C. (2012). The influence of family tradition and psychological traits on entrepreneu- rial intention. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 31, 489-499. doi:10.1016/j.ijhm.2011.07.007 Anderson, S., & Schwartz, A. (1992). Intolerance of ambiguity and depression. Social Cognition, 10, 271-298. doi:10.1521/soco.1992.10.3.271 Bardi, A., Guerra, V. M., Sharadeh, G., & Ramdeny, D. (2009). Open- ness and ambiguity intolerance: Their differential relations to well- being in the context of an academic life transition. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 219-223. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.003 Becherer, R. C., & Maurer, J. G. (1999). The proactive personality dis- position and entrepreneurial behavior among small company presi- dents. Journal of Small Business Management, 37, 28-36. Benjamin, A. J., Riggio, R. E., & Mayes, B. T. (1996). Reliability and factor structure of Budner’s Tolerance for Ambiguity Scale. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 625-632. Birrell, J., Meares, K., Wilkinson, A., & Freeston, M. (2011). Toward a definition of intolerance of uncertainty. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1198-1208. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.009 Budner, S. (1962). Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. Journal of Personality, 30, 29-50. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1962.tb02303.x Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2006). Investigating the construct validity of intolerance of uncertainty and its unique relationship with worry. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 222-236. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.12.004 Caligiuri, P. M., Jacobs, R. R., & Farr, J. L. (2000). The attitudinal and behavioural openness scale: Scale development and construct valida- tion. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 27-46. doi:10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00021-8 Caligiuri, P., & Tarique, I. (2012). Dynamic cross-cultural competen- vies and gobal leadership effectiveness. Journal of World Business, in press. doi:10.1016/j.jwb.2012.01.014 Carleton, R. N., Norton, M. A. P., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2007). Fearing the unknown: A short version of the intolerance of uncer- tainty scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 105-117. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014 Carter, J. D., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Individual differences in the accu- racy of detecting social covariations: Ecological sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 439-455. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.07.007 Chong, V. K. (1998). Testing the contingency (fit) between manage- ment accounting systems and the managerial performance: A re- search note on the moderating role of tolerance of ambiguity. British Accounting Review, 30, 331-342. doi:10.1006/bare.1998.0073 Conchar, M. P., Zinkhan, G. M., Peters, C., & Olavarrieta, S. (2004). An integrated framework for the conceptualization of consumers’ perceived risk processing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci- ence, 32, 418-436. doi:10.1177/0092070304267551 DeRoma, V. M., Martin, K. M., & Kessler, M. L. (2003). The relation- ship between tolerance for ambiguity and need for course structure. Journal of the Institute of Psychology, 30, 104-109. Diaz, F., & Rodriguez, A. (2003). Locus of control and values of com- munity entrepreneurs. Social Behavior and Personality, 31, 739-748. doi:10.2224/sbp.2003.31.8.739 Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1997). Intolerance of uncertainty and problem orientation in worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 593-606. doi:10.1023/A:1021890322153 Durrheim, K., & Foster, D. (1997). Tolerance of ambiguity as a content specific construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 741- 750. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00207-3 Ehrman, M. E. (1999). Ego boundaries and tolerance of ambiguity in second language learning. In J. Arnold (Ed.), Affect in Language Learning (pp. 68-86). Cambridge, New York. Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 75, 643-669. doi:10.2307/1884324 Endres, M. L., Chowdhury, S., & Milner, M. (2009) .Ambiguity toler- ance and accurate assessement of self-efficacy in a complex decision task. Journal of Management and Organization, 15, 31-46. doi:10.5172/jmo.837.15.1.31 Entrialgo, M., Fernandez, E., & Vazquez, C. J. (2000). Psychological characteristics and process: The role of entrepreurship in Spanish Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 725
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS SMEs. European Journal of Innovation Management, 3, 137-149. doi:10.1108/14601060010334894 Eysenck, H. (1954). The psychology of politics. London: Routledge. Facione, N. C., Facione, P. A., & Sanchez, C. A. (1994). Critical think- ing disposition as a measure of competent clinical judgment: The de- velopment of the California critical thinking disposition inventory. Journal of Nursing Education, 33, 345-350. Feather, N. (1969). Preference for information in relation to consistency, novelty, intolerance of ambiguity and dogmatism. Australian Journal of Psychology, 31, 235-249. doi:10.1080/00049536908257793 Fergus, T., & Wu, K. (2013). The intolerance of uncertainty scale. Assessment. Firoozabadi, A., & Bahredar, M. J. (2006). P02-419-Ambiguity toler- ance in students entering medical school (SHIRAZ-2006). European Psychiatry, 26, 1015. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(11)72720-1 Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why do people worry? Personality and Individual Differ- ences, 17, 791-802. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90048-5 Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1948). Intolerance of ambiguity as an emo- tional and perceptual personality variable. Journal of Personality, 18, 108-123. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1949.tb01236.x Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1949). Tolerance toward ambiguity as a per- sonality variable. American Psychologist, 3, 268. Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1951). Personality theory and perception. In R. Blake, & E. Ramsey (Eds.), Perception: An approach to personality. New York: Ronald. doi:10.1037/11505-013 Furnham, A. (1994). A content, correlational and factor-analytic study of 4 tolerance of ambiguity questionnaires. Personality and Individ- ual Differences, 16, 403-410. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90066-3 Furnham, A., & Avison, M. (1997). Personality and preference for sur- real paintings. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 923-935. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00131-1 Furnham, A., & Ribchester, T. (1995). Tolerance of ambiguity: A re- view of the concept, its measurement and applications. Current Psy- chology, 14, 179-199. doi:10.1007/BF02686907 Geller, G., Tambor, E. S., Chase, G. A., & Holtzman, N. A. (1993). Measuring physicians’ tolerance for ambiguity and its relationship to their reported practices regarding genetic testing. Medical Care, 31, 989-1001. doi:10.1097/00005650-199311000-00002 Goldratt, E. (2000). The impact of perceived uncertainty, tolerance for ambiguity and feelings of stress on employees attitudes towards or- ganizational change. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Bar-Ilan Uni- versity, Ramat Gan. Green V., & Roger, D. (2001). Coping with uncertainty: The construc- tion of a new measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 519-534. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00156-2 Grenier, S., Barrette, A. M., & Ladouceur, R. (2005). Intolerance of uncertainty and intolerance of ambiguity: Similarities and differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 593-600. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.014 Gupta, A., & Govindarajan, V. (1984). Business unit strategy, manage- rial characteristics and business unit effectiveness at strategy imple- mentation. Academy of Management Journal, 27, 25-41. doi:10.2307/255955 Gurel, E., Altinay, L., & Daniele, R. (2010). Tourism students’ entre- preneurial intentions. Annals of Tourism Research, 37, 646-669. doi:10.1016/j.annals.2009.12.003 Hartmann, F., & Slapnicar, S. (2012). The perceived fairness of per- formance evaluation: The role of uncertainty. Management Account- ing Research, 23, 17-33. doi:10.1016/j.mar.2011.10.004 Hartmann, F. G. H. (2005). The effects of tolerance for ambiguity and uncertainty on the appropriateness of accounting performance meas- ures. ABACUS, 41, 241-264. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6281.2005.00181.x Hazen, B. T., Overstreet, R. E., Jones-Farmer, L. A., & Field, H. S. (2012). The role of ambiguity tolerance in consumer perception of remanufactured products. International Journal of Production Eco- nomics, 135, 781-790. doi:10.1016/j.ijpe.2011.10.011 Herman, J. L., Stevens, M. J., Bird, A., Mendenhall, M., & Oddou, G. (2010). The tolerance for ambiguity scale: Towards a more refined measure for international management research. International Jour- nal of Intercultural Relations, 34, 58-65. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2009.09.004 Hofstede, G. (1984). Cultures’ consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hofstede, G., & Bond, M. H. (1984). Hofstede’s cultural dimensions: An independent validation using Rockeach’s value survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 417-433. doi:10.1177/0022002184015004003 Houran, J., & Williams, C. (1998). Relation of tolerance of ambiguity to global and specific paranormal experience. Psychological Reports, 83, 807-818. Hung, K.-T., & Tangpong, C. (2010). General risk propensity in multi- faceted business decisions: Scale development. Journal of Manage- rial Issues, 22, 88-106. Ironside, P. M., Jefferies, P. R., & Martin, A. (2009). Fostering patient safety competencies using multiple-patient simulation experiences. Nursing Outlook, 57, 332-337. doi:10.1016/j.outlook.2009.07.010 Iyer, G., McBride, D., & Reckers, P. (2012). The effect of a decision aid on risk aversion in capital investment decisions. Advances in Ac- counting, 28, 64-74. doi:10.1016/j.adiac.2012.02.007 Jaensch, E. (1938). Der gegenryppus. Leipzig: Barth. Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Pucik, V., & Welbourne, T. M. (1999). Managerial coping with organizational change: A dispositional per- spective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 107-122. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.1.107 Kaplan, K. J. (1972). On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic differential technique. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 361-372. doi:10.1037/h0032590 Krohne, H. W. (1989). The concept of coping modes: Relating cogni- tive person variables to actual coping behaviour. Advances in Be- haviour Research and Therapy, 11, 235-248. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(89)90027-1 Krohne, H. W. (1993). Vigilance and cognitive avoidance as concepts in coping research. In H. W. Krohne (Ed.), Attention and avoidance. Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber. Lachance, S., Ladouceur, R., & Dugas, M. J. (1999). Elements ex- plaining the tendency to worry. Applied Psychology—An Interna- tional Review, 48, 187-196. doi:10.1080/026999499377600 Ladouceur, R., Gosselin, P., & Dugas., M. J. (2000). Experimental ma- nipulation of intolerance of uncertainty: A study of a theoretical model of worry. Behaviour Therapy and Research, 38, 933-941. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00133-3 Lal, M., & Hassel, L. (1998). The joint impact of environmental uncer- tainty and tolerance of ambiguity on top managers’ perceptions of the usefulness of non-conventional management accounting informa- tion. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 14, 259-271. doi:10.1016/S0956-5221(98)80010-9 Lange, R., & Houran, J. (1998). Delusions of the paranormal: A haunt- ing question of perception. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186, 637-645. doi:10.1097/00005053-199810000-00008 Lange, R., & Houran, J. (1999). The role of fear in delusions of the paranormal. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 159-166. doi:10.1097/00005053-199903000-00005 Lauriola, M., & Levin, I. P. (2001). Relating individual differences in Attitude toward Ambiguity to risky choices. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 14, 107-122. doi:10.1002/bdm.368 Lauriola, M., Levin, I. P., & Hart, S. S. (2007). Common and distinct factors in decision making under ambiguity and risk: A psychometric study of individual differences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 130-149. DOI:10.1016/J.OBHDP.2007.04.001 Le, A., Haller, C. S., Langer, E. J., & Courvoisier, D. S. (2012). Mind- ful multitasking: The relationship between mindful flexibility and media multitasking. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1526-1532. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.03.022 Leong, C. H., & Ward, C. (2000). Identity conflict in sojourners. Inter- national Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 763-776. doi:10.1016/S0147-1767(00)00030-4 Litman, J. A. (2010). Relationships between measures of I- and D-type curiosity, ambiguity tolerance, and need for closure: An initial test of the wanting-liking model of information-seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 397-402. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.005 Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 726
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Lorsch, J. W., & Morse, J. J. (1974). Organisations and their members. New York: Harper and Row. MacDonald, A. P. (1970). Revised scale for ambiguity tolerance: Reli- ability and validity. Psychological Reports, 26, 791-798. doi:10.2466/pr0.1970.26.3.791 Marginson, D., & Ogden, S. (2005). Coping with ambiguity through the budget: The positive effects of budgetary targets on managers’ budg- eting behaviours. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 30, 435- 456. doi:10.1016/j.aos.2004.05.004 McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 323-337. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.3.323 McLain, D. L. (1993). The MSTAT-I: A new measure of an individ- ual’s tolerance for ambiguity. Educational and Psychological Meas- urement, 53, 183-189. doi:10.1177/0013164493053001020 McLain, D. L. (2009). Evidence of the properties of an ambiguity tol- erance measure: The multiple stimulus types ambiguity tolerance scale-II. Psychological Reports, 105, 975-988. doi:10.2466/pr0.105.3.975-988 McMullen, J. S., & Shepherd, D. A. ( 2006). Entrepreneurial action and the role of uncertainty in the theory of the entrepreneur. Academy of Management Review, 31, 132-152. doi:10.5465/AMR.2006.19379628 Merrotsky, P. (2013). Tolerance of ambuity: A trait of the creative per- sonality. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 232-237. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.783762 Million, T. (1957). Authoritarianism, intolerance of ambiguity and ri- gidity under ego- and task-involving conditions. Journal of Abnor- mal and Social Psychology, 99, 29-33. doi:10.1037/h0040352 Morton, K. R., Worthley, J. S., Nitch, S. R., Lamberton, H. H., Loo, L. K., & Testerman, J. K. (2000). Integration of cognition and emotion: A postformal operations model of physician-patient interaction. Jour- nal of Adult Development, 7, 151-160. doi:10.1023/A:1009542229631 Neill, S., & Rose, G. M. (2007). Achieving adaptive ends through equi- vocality: A study of organizational antecedents and consequences. Journal of Business Research, 60, 305-313. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.10.020 Norton, R. W. (1975). Measure of ambiguity tolerance. Journal of Per- sonality Assessment, 39, 607-619. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa3906_11 O’Connor, P. (1952). Ethnocentrism, “intolerance of ambiguity”, and abstract reasoning ability. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol- ogy, 47, 526-530. doi:10.1037/h0056142 Pillis, E., & Reardon, K. (2007). The influence of personality traits and persuasive messages on entrepreneurial intention. Career Develop- ment International, 12, 382-396. doi:10.1108/13620430710756762 Rajagopal, L., & Hamouz, F. L. (2009). Use of food attitudes and be- haviors in determination of the personality characteristic of openness: A pilot study. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 254-258. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2009.02.004 Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and at- tainment tests. Chicago, IL: MESA Press. Richardson, P. S., Jain, A. K., & Dick, A. (1996). Household store brand proneness: A framework. Journal of Retailing, 72, 159-185. doi:10.1016/S0022-4359(96)90012-3 Rong, G., & Grover, V. (2009). Keeping up-to-date with information technology: Testing a model of technological knowledge renewal ef- fectiveness for IT professionals. Information and Management, 46, 376-387. doi:10.1016/j.im.2009.07.002 Rush, M. C., Schoel, W. A., & Barnard, S. M. (1995). Psychological resiliency in the public sector: “Hardiness” and pressure for change. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 46, 17-39. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1995.1002 Rydell, S. (1966). Tolerance of ambiguity and semantic differential ratings. Psychological Reports, 19, 1303-1312. doi:10.2466/pr0.1966.19.3f.1303 Rydell, S. T., & Rosen, E. (1966). Measurement and some correlates of need-cognition. Psychological Reports, 19, 139-165. doi:10.2466/pr0.1966.19.1.139 Saunders, D. (1955). Some preliminary interpretive material for the PRI research memorandum SS. Educational Testing Service. Scott, W. A. (1966). Brief report: Measures of cognitive structure. Mul- tivariate Behavioral Research, 1, 391-395. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr0103_9 Scott, W. A. (1969). Structure of natural cognitions. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 12, 261-278. doi:10.1037/h0027734 Scott, W. A., Osgood, D. W., & Peterson, C. (1979). Cognitive struc- ture: Theory and measurement of individual differences. Washington: Winston & Sons. Steginga, S. K., & Occhipinti, S. (2004). The application of the heuris- tic-systematic processing model to treatment decision-making about prostate cancer. Medical Decision Making, 24, 573-583. doi:10.1177/0272989X04271044 Stewart Jr., W. H., Carland, J. C., Carland, J. W., Watson, W. E., & Sweo, R. (2003). Entrepreneurial dispositions and goal orientations: A comparative exploration of United States and Russian entrepre- neurs. Journal of Small Business Management, 41, 27-46. doi:10.1111/1540-627X.00065 Storey, R. G., & Aldag, R. J. (1983). Perceived environmental uncer- tainty: A test of an integrated explanatory model. Paper presented at the 43rd Annual meeting of the National Academy of Management. Swami, V., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2010). The dis- interested play of thought: Individual differences and preference for surrealist motion pictures. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 855-859. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.013 Tapanes, M. A., Smith, G. G., & White, J. A. (2009). Cultural diversity in online learning: A study of the perceived effects of dissonance in levels of individualism/collectivism and tolerance of ambiguity. In- ternet and Higher Education, 12, 26-34. doi:10.1016/j.iheduc.2008.12.001 Teoh, H. Y., & Foo, S. L. (1997). Moderating effects of tolerance for ambiguity and risk-taking propensity on the role conflict-perceived performance relationship: Evidence from Singaporean entrepreneurs. Journal of Business Venturing, 12, 67-81. doi:10.1016/S0883-9026(96)00035-3 Thalbourne, M. A., & Houran, J. (2000). Transliminality, the mental experience inventory and tolerance of ambiguity. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 853-863. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00143-9 Thalbourne, M. A. (1994). Belief in the paranormal and its relationship to schizophrenia-relevant measures: A confirmatory study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 78-80. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1994.tb01097.x Thalbourne, M. A., Dunbar, K. A., & Delin, P. S. (1995). An investiga- tion into correlates of belief in the paranormal. Journal of the Ameri- can Society for Psychical Research, 89, 215-231. Van Hook, C. W., & Steele, C. (2002). Individual personality charac- teristics related to suggestibility. Psychological Reports, 91, 1007- 1010. doi:10.2466/PR0.91.7.1007-1010 Weisbrod, E. (2009). The role of affect and tolerance of ambiguity in ethical decision making. Advances in Accounting, Incorporating Ad- vances in International Accounting, 25, 57-63. Westerberg, M., Singh, J., & Hackner, E. (1997). Does the CEO matter? Scandanavian Journal of Management, 13, 251-270. doi:10.1016/S0956-5221(97)00011-0 Wittenberg, K. J., & Norcross, J. C. (2001). Practitioner perfectionism: Relationship to ambiguity tolerance and work satisfaction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 1543-1550. doi:10.1002/jclp.1116 Wolfradt, U., & Dorsh, S. (1995). Schizotypy and paranormal beliefs. A cross-cultural study between German and Mongolian pupils. In R. V. Quekelberghe (Ed.), Ethnopsychology and psychotherapy. Sha- manistic healing rituals and modern therapies in comparison (pp. 228-245). Wolfradt, U., & Rademacher, J. (1999). Interpersonal intolerance of ambiguity as a clinical differential criterium. Zeitschrift for Differen- tielle und Diagnostiche Psychologie, 20, 10-20. Wolfradt, U., Oubaid, V., Straube, E. R., Bischoff, N., & Mischo, J. (1999). Thinking styles, schizotypal traits and anomalous experi- ences. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 821-830. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00031-8 Yurtsever, G. (2001). Tolerance of ambiguity, information, and nego- Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 727
 A. FURNHAM, J. MARKS Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 728 tiation. Psychological Reports, 89, 57-64. Yurtsever, G. (2008). Negotiators’ profit predicted by cognitive reap- praisal, suppression of emotions, misrepresentation of information, and tolerance of ambiguity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 590- 608. doi:10.2466/pms.106.2.590-608
|