
P. M. BJÖRN, M. KYTTÄLÄ
their family communication due to their earlier entrance into
puberty (see also Olson et al., 1983). Especially mothers and
adolescents shared an understanding of their communication
problems (see also Smith & Kerpelman, 2002). The results also
suggested,that in contrast to fathers, mothers evaluated com-
munication with their adolescent children as being very positive
(see also Campione-Barr & Smetana, 2004; Olson et al., 1983;
Schwartz, Barton-Henry, & Pruzinsky, 1985). In turn, as was
posited, the highest amount of Negative communication in
general and the lowest amount of Positive communication with
their parents was reported by the adolescents (see also Noller &
Bagi, 1985). In addition, the girls reported more negativity in
their parent-adolescent communication than did the boys. The
mothers’ presence in all of the models tells something about
modern families: the mothers remain at the communicative core
of the families. In other words, although the fathers are present
in family activities much more than they used to be, the mater-
nal role as a communicative bridge between family members
has sustained.
Secondly, the extent to which family structure is related to
adolescent, maternal and paternal views on communication was
examined. As expected, the results showed that family structure
accounted for adolescent assessment of parent-adolescent com-
munication in particular: adolescents and notably girls from
families with two-parent intact structure had evaluated their
communication as more positive according to this data than did
those adolescents with other types of families. This result sug-
gests that belonging to a family with two parents is a privilege
in the sense of expecting more open family communication
(Smith & Kerpelman, 2002; Perosa & Tam, 2006). The stability
of family structure has been observed to be related to perceived
maternal and paternal support in earlier studies, as well. For
example, Turner et al. (2004) found that children with two-
parent intact families perceived more maternal and paternal
support than the children from single-parent families. Family
structures other than two-parent families have often been ob-
served to be related to issues such as fewer economic resources
or behavior problems in adolescence (Perosa & Perosa, 1993;
Perosa et al., 1996). Feedback the individuals get from one’s
actions (LaRossa & Reitzes, 1993) either helps or constrains a
family member in finding his or her place within a family sys-
tem. This might give an explanation to the question why the
modern families still seek to communicate according to very
traditional roles: it might be the only way to search for some
kind of stability between its members (Epstein & Ward, 2011).
Conclusion
Overall, the results of the present study revealed that the
modern families in Finland still communicate according to very
traditional roles.Moreover, this finding appears to be in line
with recent findings across a variety of cultures (see, Rosnati et
al., 2011; Xiao et al., 2011) and in a variety of research settings.
Further, there was an agreement on family communication
between the adolescent and adult members regarding problems
in communication in the current data. The communication was
interpreted as being more positive in nuclear families than in
other types of families. This suggests that stability in family
structure yields positive communication outcomes in adole-
scence. However, the result does not indicate that diverse
families in the sense of their structures would not be able to
manage positive communication between its members. Instead,
with enough time, also families in change can create their own
traditions and ways to communicate.
REFERENCES
Arbuckle, J. L. (2010). A mos 18.0 user’s guide. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.
Barnes, H. L., & Olson, D. L. (1982). Parent-adolescent communi-
cation scale. In D. H. Olson et al. (Eds.), Family inventories: Inven-
tories used in a national survey of families across the family life cy-
cle (pp. 33-48). St Paul: Family Social Science, University of Min-
nesota.
Barnes, H. L., & Olson, D. L. (1985). Parent-adolescent communication
and the circumplex model. Child Development, 56, 438-447.
Björn, P. M., & Kyttälä, M. (2011). Family structure and academic
skills among Finnish adolescents. European Journal of Psychology
of Education, 26, 465-477. doi:10.1007/s10212-011-0058-5
Callan, V. J., & Noller, P. (1986). Perceptions of communicative rela-
tionships in families with adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 48, 813-820. doi:10.2307/352574
Campione-Barr, N., & Smetana, J. G. (2004). In the eye of the beholder:
Subjective and observer ratings of middle-class African-American
mother-adolescent interactions. Developmental Psychology, 40, 927-
947. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.927
De Los Reyes, A., Goodman, K. L., Kliewer, W., & Reid-Qiñ Ones, K.
R. (2010). The longitudinal concistency of mother-child reporting
discrepancies of parental monitoring and their ability to predict child
delinquent behavior two years later. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
cence, 39, 1417-1430. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9496-7
Epstein, M., & Ward, L. (2011). Exploring parent-adolescent commu-
nication about gender: Results from adolescent and emerging adult
samples. Sex Roles, 65, 108-118. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9975-7
Faber, A. J., Edwards, A. E., Bauer, K. S., & Wetchler, J. L. (2003).
Family structure: Its effects on adolescent attachment and identity
formation. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 31, 243-255.
doi:10.1080/01926180390201945
Finnish statistical centre (2011).
http://tilastokeskus.fi/til/perh/2010/perh_2010_2011-0527_tie_001_fi
.html
Hauser, S. T., Book, B. K., Houlihan, J., Powers, S., Weiss-Perry, B.,
Follansbee, & Noam, G. G. (1987). Sex differences within the family:
Studies of adolescent and parent-family interactions. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 3, 199-220. doi:10.1007/BF02139091
Heller, S. R., Robinson, L. C., Henry, C. S., & Plunkett, S. W. (2006).
Gender differences in adolescent perceptions of parent-adolescent
openness in communication and adolescent empathy. Marriage &
Family Review, 40, 103-122. doi:10.1300/J002v40n04_06
Jallinoja, R., & Widmer, E. D. (2011). Families and kinship in con-
temporary Europe: Rules and practices of relatedness. Basingstoke:
Palgrave Macmillan. doi:10.1057/9780230307452
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O., & Elder Jr., G. (2001). Parent-
adolescent reciprocity in negative affect and its relation to early
adult social development. Developmental Psychology, 37, 775-790.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.775
Kyttälä, M., & Björn, P. M. (2010). Prior mathematics achievement,
cognitive appraisals and anxiety as predictors of Finnish students’
later mathematics performance and career orientation. Educational
Psychology, 4, 431-448. doi:10.1080/01443411003724491
LaRossa, R., & Reitzes, D. C. (1993). Symbolic interactionism and fa-
mily studies. In P. G. Doherty, W. J. LaRossa, R. Schumm., & S. K.
Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theories and methods. New
York: Plenum Press, 135-163. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-85764-0_6
Noller, P., & Bagi, S. (1985). Parent-adolescent communication. Jour-
nal of Adolescence, 8, 125-144. doi:10.1016/S0140-1971(85)80042-7
Noller, P., & Callan, V. (1988). Understanding parent-adolescent inter-
actions: Perceptions of family members and outsiders. Developmen-
tal Psychology, 24, 707-714. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.5.707
Olson, D. H., McCubbin, H. I., Barnes, H. L., Larsen, A. S., Muxen, M.
J., & Wilson, M. A. (1983). Families: What makes them work. Bev-
erly Hills, CA: Sage.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes.
236