
K. M. GADZICHOWSKI
English language they no longer feel insecure and are likely to
contemplate the answer choices without being self-conscious.
In 2006, Hendricks, Trueblood, and Pasnak expanded the
depth of patterning instruction to include 480 color, size, num-
ber, letter, and time patterns ranging from alternations similar
to those of Herman (1973) to complex patterns with varying
numbers of steps between items presented in matrices. The
results from their study indicated that the children who received
this type of instruction made greater academic gains than chil-
dren in their control groups, who had received instruction fo-
cused specifically on academic material that had been devel-
oped upon the recommendation of teachers. The children’s
scores on the DAB Total Achievement measure were correlated
with the patterning test scores, r(60) = .40 p < .01. Correlations
for the DAB’s subscales were similar: ma thematics r(60) = .42,
p < .01, written language, r(60) = .35, p < .01, spoken language,
r(60) = .38, p < .01 These are considered to be medium effect
sizes (Cohen, 1992).
There were no group differences on the DAB scale. However,
in spite of random assignment of participants to groups, there
was a 5 point difference between the groups on average intelli-
gence, favoring the control group, and unequal variances. Be-
cause of this, a MANCOVA was conducted and the SIT-R
scores were used as a covariate. Once the groups were equated
statistically on the SIT-R, the patterning group did display im-
provement that was significantly more than the improvement
made by the control group on the DAB Total Achievement
measure.
These three studies constitute the entire set of empirical evi-
dence that instruction in patterning has an effect beyond help-
ing children become better at patterning per se. However, it is
clear that patterning instruction has become a fixture in educa-
tion, and for that reason deserves investigation.
To date, no research has been published in the professional
literature that was designed to determine which types of pat-
terns are the easiest or hardest for children to learn. However,
Boyer, Sweeting, Pasnak and Kidd (2010) presented a poster
indicating that the number of items a pattern “skipped” was
relevant. First grade children were most accurate on patterns
that skipped a single item, followed by patterns that skipped
two items and finally, patterns that skipped three items.
For younger (preschool) children, research involving oddity
problems showed that the dimension (color, form, size, and
orientation) in which it was presented was relevant in deter-
mining ease or difficulty of an oddity problem. Color oddity
problems were easier than form oddity and orientation oddity
problems (Gadzichowski & Pasnak, 2010). Since the dimension
of the oddity problem did impact how easy the problem was for
the younger children (age 4), it is an area of interest when as-
sessing patterning ability in older children. Perhaps the dimen-
sion in which a pattern is presented affects how difficult it is to
understand the pattern. Boyer, Sweeting, Kidd, and Pasnak
(2010) reported that patterns of letters were easier for children
than patterns of time as represented by clock faces, and patterns
of numbers were harder than patterns of rotation, time and let-
ters. However, their design was nonfactorial and incomplete,
and Gadzichowski, Kidd, Pasnak, and Boyer (2010) reported
that these dimensions did not produce significant differences,
so the issue is only partially resolved.
Developmental psychologists have neglected patterning in
their study of cognition. There is no theory that puts patterning
among the cognitive tasks mastered during a child’s develop-
ment. Furthermore, there is no understanding or explanation of
how failure to master this cognitive ability would detract from a
child’s ability to master more abstract cognitive concepts. How-
ever, children transition from one type of thinking in preschool
to a more complex cognition in early elementary school—the
transition from preoperational to concrete operational thought
(Piaget, 1963/1936)—and patterning incorporates many of the
changes in reasoning ability made during this time. Patterning
may be closely related to transitivity, an ability in which some-
one is capable of deducing the relationship between two items
by examining how those items relate to a third item. Clements
and Sarama (2007) found that having the ability to make indi-
rect comparisons, along with the ability to order objects ac-
cording to their size (seriation) are cognitive skills that are nec-
essary for successful performance in elementary school. “In-
deed, research indicates that mastering these skills is one of the
strongest predictors of school success or failure” (Clements &
Sarama, 2008: p. 365). In seriation, a child must understand
how an item relates to the items that come before or after it in
the sequence. When looking at patterns, this same rule applies.
In seriation, the relation is a simple one. The item in question is
either smaller or larger than the neighboring items on some
dimension (height, width, weight, length, or overall size). In
patterning the relation is more complex; the relation might in-
volve size, color, size and color, shape, or other dimensions that
are more abstract, and the relationships can be much more
complicated.
Transitivity is the understanding that if item A is related to
item B in some way, and item B is related to item C in some
way, then the relationship between A and C can be determined
by comparing these items to item B. The relationship between
A and C (the key items) is not directly observed but rather de-
duced by comparing the key items to another item. Both transi-
tivity and patterning incorporate the idea that an item is defined
by, and simultaneously defines, properties of items that follow
or precede it. The primary difference between transitivity and
patterning is that patterning does not require an individual to
utilize the relations of A to B and C to B in order to determine
the relation of A to C. Transitivity does require that one use
those relations. Since a child could make use of the transitive
relation or alternatively make use of the simultaneous presenta-
tion of all the items and compare A to C directly, perhaps pat-
terning is a precursor to transitivity.
Examining the differences between patterns wherein the
missing item occurs at the beginning, middle or end may offer
some insight into the relationship between patterning and tran-
sitivity. Children who are capable of solving patterns that are
missing the beginning or end item may have begun to master
transitivity, because they must infer the nature of the missing
item from one that just precedes or follows it. When children
can rely on the presence of items on both sides of a missing
item (patterns missing a middle object) to determine the nature
of the missing item, they may be reversing the transitivity op-
eration. Thus, when a child understands patterns of items in
which some items follow and precede other items, based on the
rule of that pattern, then the child can make inferences about a
neighboring item by looking at any one item in the pattern. A
more advanced extrapolation would be to use a single item to
make inferences about both of the neighboring items succes-
sively or simultaneously. The ability to compare the two infer-
ences about the neighboring items in order to relate the neigh-
boring items to one another would be considered transitivity.
Copyright © 2012 SciRes.
1178