Cognitive Demand and Reminders Effect on Time-Based Prospective Memory in Amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment (AMCI) and in Healthy Elderly

Abstract

Individuals with-Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) often complain of difficulty remembering to carry out intended actions. We investigated the relative efficacy of a different reminder in performing a time-based Prospective Memory (PM) task. The PM performance of 24 participants with amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment (AMCI) has been compared with that of 24 healthy controls. As ongoing task, samples of the Attentive Matrices Test were used. In the PM task subjects were requested to write an “X” every three minutes during a 9 minutes period. Participants received the task consisting either in a low demand condition (checking number “5”) or in a high demand condition (checking numbers “1”, “4”, “9”). In order to be as punctual as possible, participants were asked to simultaneously write the “X” at task time expiration, using a digital clock. Time monitoring was recorded. Reminder occurring was manipulated in that participants could receive critical, accidental or completely absent reminder. As expected, high cognitive demand was negatively correlated with PM performance and time monitoring. Unexpectedly, all the participants did not benefit from the critical reminder. These findings demonstrated, from a behavioral perspective, that Working Memory (WM) and PM processes are not based on the same memory system and PM may require WM resources at high demand.

Share and Cite:

O. Pino, F. Poletti and P. Caffarra, "Cognitive Demand and Reminders Effect on Time-Based Prospective Memory in Amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment (AMCI) and in Healthy Elderly," Open Journal of Medical Psychology, Vol. 2 No. 1, 2013, pp. 35-46. doi: 10.4236/ojmp.2013.21007.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Prospective Memory: Theory and Application,” Lawrence Erlbaum Associated, Mahwah, 1996.
[2] M. A. McDaniel and G. O. Einstein, “The Importance of Cue Familiarity and Cue Distinctiveness in Prospective Memory,” Memory, Vol. 1, No. 1, 1993, pp. 23-41. doi:10.1080/09658219308258223
[3] G. J. Kinsella, E. Mullaly, E. Rand, B. Ong, C. Burton, S. Price, M. Phillips and E. Storey, “Early Intervention for Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Randomised Controlled Trial,” Journal of Neurological and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, Vol. 80, No. 7, 2009, pp. 730-736. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.148346
[4] C. M. Will, P. G. Rendell, S. Ozgis, J. M. Pierson, B. Ong and J. D. Henry, “Cognitively Impaired Older Adults Exhibit Comparable Difficulties on Naturalistic and Laboratory Prospective Memory Tasks,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 23, No. 6, 2009, pp. 804-812. doi:10.1002/acp.1514
[5] T. Shallice and P. W. Burgess, “Deficits in Strategy Application Following Frontal Lobe Damage in Man,” Brain, Vol. 114, No. 2, 1991, pp. 727-741. doi:10.1093/brain/114.2.727
[6] R. Petersen, G. Smith, S. Waring, et al., “Mild Cognitive Impairment. Clinical Characterization and Outcome,” Archives of Neurology, Vol. 56, No. 3, 1999, pp. 303-308. doi:10.1001/archneur.56.3.303
[7] J. Ellis, “Prospective Memory of the Realization of Delayed Intentions: A Conceptual Framework for Research,” In: M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, Eds., Prospective Memory: Theory and Application, Lawrence Erlbaum Associated, Mahwah, 1996, pp. 1-22.
[8] G. O.Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Normal Aging and Prospective Memory,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 16, No. 4, 1990, pp. 717-726. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717
[9] G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, S. L. Richardson, M. J. Guynn and A. R. Cunfer, “Aging and Prospective Memory: Examining the Influences of Self-Initiated Retrieval Processes,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memoand Cognition, Vol. 21, No. 4, 1995, pp. 996-1007. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.996
[10] E. A. Maylor, G. Smith, S. Della Sala and R. H. Logie, “Prospective and Retrospective Memory in Normal Aging and Dementia: An Experimental Study,” Memory and Cognition, Vol. 30, No. 6, 2002, pp. 871-884. doi:10.3758/BF03195773
[11] F. I. M. Craik, “A Functional Account of Age Differences in Memory,” In: F. Klix and H. Hagendorf, Eds., Human Memory and Cognitive Capabilities: Mechanisms and Performances, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1986, pp. 409-422.
[12] J. E. Harris and A. J. Wilkins, “Remembering to Do This: A Theoretical Framework and an Illustrated Experiment,” Human Learning, Vol. 1, 1982, pp. 123-126.
[13] G. O. Einstein, L. J. Holland, M. A. McDaniel and M. J. Guynn, “Age-Related Deficits in Prospective Memory: The Influence of Task Complexity,” Psychology and Aging, Vol. 7, No. 3, 1992, pp. 471-478. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.471
[14] E. A. Maylor, “Age and Prospective Memory,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 42, No. 3, 1990, pp. 471-493. doi:10.1080/14640749008401233
[15] T. Goschke and J. Kuhl, “The Representation of Intentions: Persisting Activation in Memory,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 19, No. 5, 1993, pp. 1211-1226. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1211
[16] J. Ellis and A. Milne, “Retrieval Cue Specificity and the Realization of Delayed Intentions,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 49, No. 4, 1996, pp. 862-887.
[17] R. L. Marsh and J. L. Hicks, “Event-Based Prospective Memory and Executive Control of Working Memory,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 24, No. 2, 1998, pp. 336-349. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.336
[18] T. Goschke and J. Kuhl, “Remembering What to Do: Explicit and Implicit Memory for Intentions,” In: M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, Eds., Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications, Erlbaum, Mahwah, 1996, pp. 53-91.
[19] J. Freeman and J. Ellis, “Aging and the Accessibility of Performed and to-Be-Performed Actions,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, Vol. 10, No. 4, 2003, pp. 298-309. doi:10.1076/anec.10.4.298.28975
[20] P. W. Burgess, A. Quayle and C. D. Frith, “Brain Regions Involved in Prospective Memory as Determined by Positron Emission Tomography,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 39, No. 6, 2001, pp. 545-555. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00149-4
[21] R. H. Logie, E. A. Maylor, S. Della Sala and G. Smith, “Working Memory in Event- and Time-Based Prospective Memory Tasks: Effects of Secondary Demand and Age,” European Journal of cognitive psychology, Vol. 16, No. 3, 2004, pp. 441-456. doi:10.1080/09541440340000114
[22] R. L. Marsh, J. L. Hicks and M. L. Bink, “Activation of Completed, Uncompleted, and Partially Completed Intentions,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 24, No. 2, 1998, pp. 350-361. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.2.350
[23] A. D. Baddeley, “Exploring the Central Executive,” Quaterly of Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 49, No. 1, 1996, pp. 5-28.
[24] D. Basso, M. Ferrari and P. Palladino, “Prospective Memory and Working Memory: Asymmetrical Effects during Frontal Lobe TMS Stimulation,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 48, No. 11, 2010, pp. 382-290.
[25] G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, S. L. Richardson, M. J. Guynn and A. R. Cunfer, “Aging and Prospective Memory: Examining the Influences of Self-Initiated Retrieval Processes,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 21, No. 4, 1995, pp. 996-1007. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.996
[26] O. U. Vortac, M. B. Edwards and C. A. Manning, “Function of External Cues in Prospective Memory,” Memory, Vol. 3, No. 2, 1995, pp. 201-219. doi:10.1080/09658219508258966
[27] G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Normal Aging and Prospective Memory,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 16, No. 4, 1990, pp. 717-726. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717
[28] M. J. Guynn, M. A.McDaniel and G. O. Einstein, “Prospective Memory: when Reminder Fail,” Memory and Cognition, Vol. 26, No. 2, 1998, pp. 287-298. doi:10.3758/BF03201140
[29] E. A. Maylor, “Age-related Impairment in an Event-Based Prospective Memory Task,” Psychological Aging, Vol. 11, No. 1, 1996, pp. 74-78. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.74
[30] G. I. Cook, R. L. Marsh and J. L. Hicks, “Halo and Devil Effects Demonstrate Valence-Based Influences on Source-Monitoring Decisions,” Consciousness and Cognition, Vol. 12, No. 2, 2003, pp. 257-278. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00073-9
[31] M. S. Albert and D. Blacker, “Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia,” Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 2, 2006, pp. 379-388. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144039
[32] P. W. Burgess, J. S. Simons, I. Dumontheil and S. J. Gilbert, “The Gateway Hypothesis of Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10) Function,” In: J. Duncan, P. McLeod and L. Phillps, Eds., Measuring the Mind: Speed, Control and Age, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2005, pp. 217-248. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198566427.003.0009
[33] P. W. Burgess, S. K. Scott and C. D. Frith, “The Role of the Rostral Frontal Cortex (Area 10) in Prospective Memory: A Lateral versus Medial Dissociation,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 41, No. 8, 2003, pp. 906-918. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00327-5
[34] J. Okuda, T. Fujii, A. Yamadori, R. Kawashima, T. Tsukiura, R. Fukatsu, et al., “Partecipation of the Prefrontal Cortices in Prospective Memory: Evidence from a PET Study in Humans,” Neuroscience Letters, Vol. 253, No. 2, 1998, pp. 127-130. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00628-4
[35] J. S. Simons, M. L. Sch?lvinck, S. J. Gilbert, C. D. Frith and P. W. Burgess, “Differential Components of Prospective Memory? Evidence from FMRI,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 44, No. 8, 2006, pp. 1388-1397. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.005
[36] J. Poppenk, M. Moscovitch, A. R. McIntosh, E. Ozcelik and F. I. Craik, “Encoding the Future: Successful Processing of Intentions Engages Predictive Brain Networks,” Neuroimage, Vol. 49, No. 1, 2010, pp. 905-913. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.049
[37] H. Cheng, Y. Tian, P. Hu, J. Wong and K. Wang, “Time-Based Prospective Memory Impairments in Patients with Thalamic Stroke,” Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 124, No. 1, 2010, pp. 152-158. doi:10.1037/a0018306
[38] S. J. Gilbert, P. M. Gollwizer, A. L. Cohen, G. Oettingen, “Separable Brain Systems Supporting Cued versus Self-Initiated Realization of Delayed Intentions,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, Vol. 35, No. 4, 2009, pp. 905-915. doi:10.1037/a0015535
[39] E. Volle, G. Goneen-Yaacovi, A. de Lacy Costello, S. J. Gilbert and P. W. Burgess, “The Role of Rostrale Prefrontal Cortex in Prospective Memory: A Voxel-Based Lesion Study,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 49, No. 8, 2011, pp. 2189-2198. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.045
[40] P. W. Burgess and T. Shallice, “The Relationship between Prospective and Retrospective Memory: Neuropsychological Evidence,” In: M. M. Conway, Ed., Cognitive Models of Memory, MIT Press, Cambridge, 1997.
[41] M. A. Brandimote, G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications,” Erlbaum, Hillsdale, 1996.
[42] M. Kliegel, M. J. Guynn and H. Zimmer, “The Role of Noticing in Prospective Memory Forgetting,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, Vol. 64, No. 3, 2006, pp. 226-232.
[43] A. L. Cohen, R. West and F. I. Craik, “Modulation of the Prospective and Retrospective Components of Memory for Intentions in Younger and Older Adults,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, Vol. 8, No. 1, 2001, pp. 1-13. doi:10.1076/anec.8.1.1.845
[44] E. E. Smith and J. Jonides, “Storage and Executive Processes in the Frontal Lobes,” Science, Vol. 283, No. 5408, 1999, pp. 1657-1661. doi:10.1126/science.283.5408.1657
[45] M. J. Guynn, “A Two-Process Model of Strategic Monitoring in Event-Based Prospective Memory: Activation/Retrieval Mode and Checking,” International Journal of Psychology, Vol. 38, No. 4, 2003, pp. 245-256. doi:10.1080/00207590344000178
[46] A. R. Doobs and B. G. Rule, “Prospective Memory and Self-Report of Memory Abilities,” Canadian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 41, No. 2, 1987, pp. 209-222. doi:10.1037/h0084152
[47] P. W. Burgess, A. Quayle and C. D. Frith, “Brain Regions Involved in Prospective Memory as Determined by Positron Emis-Sion Tomography,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 39, No. 6, 2001, pp. 545-555. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00149-4
[48] E. A. Maylor, “Prospective Memory in Normal Ageing and Dementia,” Neurocase, Vol. 1, No. 3, 1995, pp. 285-289. doi:10.1080/13554799508402372
[49] J. A. Meacham and B. Leiman, “Remembering to Perform Future Actions,” In: U. Neisser, Ed., Memory observed: Remembering in Natural Contexts, Freeman, San Francisco, 1982, pp. 327-336.
[50] M. Moscovitch, “Memory and Working with Memory: Evaluation of a Component Process model and Comparisons with Other Models” In: D. L. Shacter, and E. Tulving, Eds., Memory systems, MT Press, Cambridge, 1994, pp. 269-310.
[51] F. Collette, M. Van der Linden and E. Salmon, “Executive Dysfunction in Alzheimer’s Disease,” Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 35, No. 1, 1999, pp. 57-72.
[52] A. Blanco-Campal, R. F. Coen, B. A. Lawlor, J. B. Walsh and T. E. Burke, “Detection of Prospective Memory Deficits in Mild Cognitive Impairment of Suspected Alzheimer’s Disease Etiology Using a Novel Event-Based Prospective Memory Task,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 15, No. 1, 2009, pp. 154-159. doi:10.1017/S1355617708090127
[53] H. Kazui, A. Matsuda, N. Hirono, E. Mori, N. Miyoshi, A. Ogino, H. Tokunaga, Y. Ikejiri and M. Takeda, “Everyday Memory Impairment of Patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment,” Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, Vol. 19, No. 5-6, 2005, pp. 331-337. doi:10.1159/000084559
[54] A. Costa, G. A. Carlesimo and C. Caltagirone, “Prospective Memory Functioning: New Area of Investigation in the Clinical Neuropsychogy and Rehabilitation of Parkinson’s Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment. Review of Evidence,” Neurological Studies, Vol. 33, No. 5, 2012, pp. 965-972.
[55] S. Karantzoulis, A. Tryer and J. B. Rich, “Prospective Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 15, No. 3, 2009, pp. 407-415. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090596
[56] J. D. Greene, J. R. Hodges and A. D. Baddeley, “Autobiographical Memory and Executive Function in Early Dementia of Alzheimer Type,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 33, No. 12, 1995, pp. 1647-1670. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00046-1
[57] A. K.Troyer and K. J. Murphy, “Memory for Intentions in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Time- and Event-Based Pro-Spective Memory,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 13, No. 2, 2007, pp. 365-369. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070452
[58] R. L. Marsh, J. L. Hicks and G. I. Cooks, “On the Relationship between Effort toward an Ongoing Task and Cue Detection in Event-Based Prospective Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 31, No. 1, 2005, pp. 68-75. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.68
[59] J. H. Kramer, A. Nelson, J. K. Johnson, K. Yaffe, S. Glenn, H. J. Rosen and B. L. Miller, “Multiple Cognitive Deficits in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment,” Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, Vol. 22, No. 4, 2006, pp. 306-311. doi:10.1159/000095303
[60] G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Prospective Memory and What Costs Do Not Reveal about Retrieval Processes. A Com-Mentary on Smith, Hunt, Mcvay, and Mcconnell (2007),” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, Vol. 36, No. 4, 2008, pp. 1082-1088. doi:10.1037/a0019184
[61] M. J. Guynn, “Theory of Monitoring in Prospective Memory: Instantiating Retrieval Mode and Periodic Target Checking,” In: M. Kliegel, M. A.McDaniel and G. O. Einstein, Eds., Perspective Memory: Cognitive, Neuroscience, Developmental and Applied Perspective,” Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York, 2008, pp. 53-76.
[62] G. Cona, G. Arcara, V. Tarantino, P. A. Bisiacchi, “Electrophysiological Correlates of Strategic Monitoring in Event-Based and Time-Based Prospective Memory,” PloS One, Vol. 7, No. 2, 2012, p. e31659.
[63] R. Perri, G. A. Carlesimo, L. Serra and C. Caltagirone, “Characterization of Memory Profile in Subjects with Mild Cognitive Impairment,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Vol. 27, No. 8, 2005, pp. 1033-1055. doi:10.1080/13803390490919317
[64] A. S. Zigmond and R. P. Snaith, “The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale,” Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Vol. 67, No. 6, 1983, pp. 361-370. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
[65] R. C. Petersen, “Mild Cognitive Impairment as a Diagnostic Entity,” Journal of Internal Medicine, Vol. 256, No. 3, 2004, pp. 183-194. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x
[66] A. Orsini, D. Grossi, E. Capitani, M. Laiacona, C. Papagno and G. Vallar, “Verbal and Spatial Immediate Memory Span; Normative Data from 1355 Adults and 1112 Children,” Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, Vol. 8, No. 6, 1987, pp. 539-48. doi:10.1007/BF02333660
[67] J. C. Raven, “Progressive Matrices,” Lewis, London, 1947.
[68] D. Wechsler, “Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III,” Psychological Corporation, New York, 1997.
[69] S. Della Sala, A. Baddeley, C. Papagno and H. Spinnler, “Dual task paradigm: A Means to Examine the Central Executive,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 769, No. 1, 1995, pp. 161-172. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38137.x
[70] P. Caffarra, G. Vezzadini, F. Dieci, A. Zonato, and A. Venneri, “Una Versione Abbreviata Del Test di Stroop: Dati Normativi Nella Popolazione Italiana,” Nuova Rivista di Neurologia, Vol. 12, No. 4, 2002, pp. 111-115
[71] R. Krikorian, J. Bartok and N. Gay, “Tower of London Procedure: A Standard Method and Developmental Data,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Vol. 16, No. 6, 1994, pp. 840-850. doi:10.1080/01688639408402697
[72] G. A. Carlesimo, C. Caltagirone and G. Gainotti, “The Mental Deterioration Battery: Normative Data, Diagnostic Reliability and Qualitative Analyses of Cognitive Impairment. The Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery,” European Journal of Neurology, Vol. 36, No. 6, 1996, pp. 378-384. doi:10.1159/000117297
[73] G. Novelli, et al., “Three Clinical Tests for the Assessment of Verbal Long-Term Memory Function: Norms from 320 Normal Subjects,” Archivio di Psicologia Neurologia e Psichiatria, Vol. 47, No. 2, 1986, pp. 278-296.
[74] P. Caffarra, G. Vezzadini, F. Dieci, A. Zonato, and A. Venneri, “Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Normative Values in an Italian Population Sample,” Neurological Science, Vol. 22, No. 6, 2002, pp. 443-447.
[75] G. O. Einstein and M. A. McDaniel, “Strategic and Automatic Processes in Prospective Memory Retrieval: A Multi-Process Framework,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 14, No. 7, 2000, pp. 127-144. doi:10.1002/acp.775
[76] H. Spinnler and G. Tognoni, “Standardizzazione e Taratura Italiana di Test Neuropsicologici,” The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, Vol. S8, No. 6, 1987, pp. 44-46.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.