[1]
|
Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 12, 385-390.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00371
|
[2]
|
Aronson, J., Fried, D. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113-125. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1491
|
[3]
|
Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29-46. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1371
|
[4]
|
Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327-344.
doi:10.1007/BF00287729
|
[5]
|
Balentine, C. B., & Brownlow, S. (2006). Does making salient task relevance to group affiliation decrease the performance of men athletes on spatial tasks? Psi Chi Journal, 11, 37-44.
|
[6]
|
Bodner, G. M., & Guay, R. B. (1997). The purdue visualization of rotations test. The Chemical Educator, 2, 118.
doi:10.1007/s00897970138a
|
[7]
|
Brodish, A. B., & Devine, P. G. (2009). The role of performance-avoidance goals and worry in mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 180-185. doi:10.1016/j/jesp.2008.08.005
|
[8]
|
Brown, R. P., & Josephs, R. A. (1999). A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 246-257.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.246
|
[9]
|
Brownlow, S., McPheron, T. K., & Acks, C. N. (2003). Science background and spatial abilities in men and women. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 12, 371-380.
doi:10.1023/B:JOST.0000006297.90536.7c
|
[10]
|
Brownlow, S., & Miderski, C. A. (2002). How gender and college chemistry experience influence mental rotation abilities. Themes in Education, 3, 133-140.
|
[11]
|
Brownlow, S., Valentine, S. E., & Owusu, A. (2008). Women athletes’ mental rotation under stereotypic threat. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107, 307-336. doi:10.2466/pms.107.1.307-316
|
[12]
|
Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Frigerio, S., Impagliazzo, L., & Latinotti, S. (2003). Stereotype threat: The effect of expectancy on performance. European Journal of Psychology, 33, 267-285.
|
[13]
|
Carr, P. B., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Stereotype threat and inflexible perseverance in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 853-859. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.003
|
[14]
|
Cheryan, S., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). When positive stereotypes threaten intellectual performance: The psychological hazards of “model minority” status. Psychological Science, 11, 399-402.
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00277
|
[15]
|
Cherney, I. D. (2008). Mom, let me play more computer games: They improve my mental rotation skills. Sex Roles, 59, 776-786.
doi:10.1007/s11199-008-9498-z
|
[16]
|
Cherney, I. D., & Collaer, M. L. (2005). Sex differences in line judgment: Relationship to mathematics preparation and strategy use. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, 615-627.
doi:10.2466/PMS.100.3.615-627
|
[17]
|
Croizet, J.-C., & Claire, T. (1998). Extending the concept of stereotype threat to social class: The intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 588-594. doi:10.1177/0146167298246003
|
[18]
|
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1615-1628.
doi:10.1177/014616702237644
|
[19]
|
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103-127.
|
[20]
|
doi:10.1177/014616702237644
|
[21]
|
Goldstein, D., Haldane, D., & Mitchell, C. (1990). Sex differences in visual-spatial ability: The role of performance factors. Memory and Cognition, 18, 546-550. doi:10.3758/BF03198487
|
[22]
|
Gonzalez, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002). The effects of stereotype threat and double minority status on the test performance of Latino women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 656-670.
|
[23]
|
Gresky, D. M., Ten Eyck, L. L., Lord, C. G., & McIntyre, R. B. (2005). Effects of salient multiple identities on women’s performance under mathematics stereotype threat. Sex Roles, 53, 703-716.
doi:10.1007/s11199-005-7735-2
|
[24]
|
Kass, S. J., Ahlers, R. H., & Dugger, M. (1998). Eliminating gender differences through practice in an applied visual space task. Human Performance, 11, 337-349. doi:10.1207/s15327043hup1104_3
|
[25]
|
Keller, J. (2002). Blatant stereotype threat and women’s math performance: Self-handicapping as a strategic means to cope with obtrusive negative performance expectations. Sex Roles, 47, 193-198.
doi:10.1023/A:1021003307511
|
[26]
|
Keller, J., & Dauenheimer, D. (2003). Stereotype threat in the classroom: Dejection mediates the disrupting threat effect on women’s math performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 371-381. doi:10.1177/0146167202250218
|
[27]
|
Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2005). Stereotype threat in men on a test of social sensitivity. Sex Roles, 52, 489-496.
doi:10.1007/s11199-005-3714-x
|
[28]
|
Lesko, A. C., & Corpus, J. H. (2006). Discounting the difficult: How high math-identified women respond to stereotype threat. Sex Roles, 54, 113-125. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-8873-2
|
[29]
|
Marx, D. M., & Roman, J. S. (2002). Female role models: Protecting women’s math test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1183-1193. doi:10.1177/01461672022812004
|
[30]
|
McIntyre, R. B., Paulson, R. M., & Lord, C. G. (2003). Alleviating women’s mathematic stereotype threat through salience of group achievements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 83-90.
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00513-9
|
[31]
|
Newcombe, N., Bandura, M. M., & Taylor, D. G. (1983). Sex differences in spatial ability and spatial activities. Sex Roles, 9, 377-385.
doi:10.1007/BF00289672
|
[32]
|
O’Brien, L. T., & Crandall, C. S. (2003). Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s math performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 782-289.
|
[33]
|
Oswald, D. L., & Lindstedt, K. (2006). The content and function of gender self-stereotypes: An exploratory investigation. Sex Roles, 54, 447-458. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9026-y
|
[34]
|
Resnick, S. M. (1993). Sex differences in mental rotations: An effect of time limits? Brain and Cognition, 21, 71-79.
doi:10.1006/brcg.1993.1005
|
[35]
|
Roberts, J. A., & Bell, M. A. (2000). Sex differences on a computerized mental rotation task disappear with computer familiarization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 1027-1034.
doi:10.2466/PMS.91.7.1027-1034
|
[36]
|
Scali, R. M., Brownlow, S., & Hicks, J. L. (2000). Gender differences in spatial task performance as a function of speed or accuracy orientation. Sex Roles, 43, 359-376. doi:10.1023/A:1026699310308
|
[37]
|
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194-201. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1500
|
[38]
|
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50, 835-850.
doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000029101.74557.a0
|
[39]
|
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336-356. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336
|
[40]
|
Seibt, B. F., & F?rster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38-56.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38
|
[41]
|
Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, D. (2003). Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 68-74.
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00508-5
|
[42]
|
Shapiro, J. R. (2011). Different groups, different threats: A mulit-threat approach to the experience of stereotype threats. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 464-480.
doi:10:1177/0146167211398140
|
[43]
|
Shapiro, J. R., & Neuberg, S. L. (2007). From stereotype threat to stereotype threats: Implications of a multi-threat framework for causes, moderators, mediators, consequences, and interventions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 107-130.
doi:10.1177/1088868306294790
|
[44]
|
Sharps, M. J., Price, J. L., & Williams, J. K. (1994). Spatial cognition and gender: Instructional and stimulus influences on mental image rotation performance. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 413-425.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb00464.x
|
[45]
|
Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, A. J., Fujita, K., & Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 638-647. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.638
|
[46]
|
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80-83. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00111
|
[47]
|
Smith, J. L. (2006). The interplay among stereotypes, performance-avoidance goals, and women’s math performance expectations. Sex Roles, 54, 287-296. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9345-z
|
[48]
|
Smith, J. L., & White, P. H. (2002). An examination of implicitly activated, explicitly activated, and nullified stereotypes on mathematical performance: It’s not just a women’s issue. Sex Roles, 47, 179-192.
doi:10.1023/A:1021051223441
|
[49]
|
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4-28. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373
|
[50]
|
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613-629.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
|
[51]
|
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. A. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
|
[52]
|
Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding practice: The effects of stereotype threat on self-handicapping in white athletes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1667-1678.
doi:10.1177/014616702237648
|
[53]
|
Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects of Black and White athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1213-1227.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213
|
[54]
|
Voyer, D., & Isaacs, M. (1993, July). Sex differences in mental rotation: Role of practice and experience. Presented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior, and Cognitive Science, Toronto, Canada.
|
[55]
|
Voyer, D., Nolan, C., & Voyer, S. (2000). The relation between everyday experience and spatial performance in men and women. Sex Roles, 43, 891-915. doi:10.1023/A:1011041006679
|
[56]
|
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250-270.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250
|
[57]
|
Walter, K. D., Roberts, A. E., & Brownlow, S. (2000). Sex differences in mental rotation and other spatial abilities as measured through transcranial doppler sonography. Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 37-45. doi:10.1027//0269-8803.14.1.37
|
[58]
|
Wraga, M., Helt, M., Jacobs, E., & Sullivan, K. (2006). Neural basis of stereotype-induced shifts in women’s mental rotation performance. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 12-19. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl041
|