[1]
|
Atmanspacher, H. (2012). Dual-aspect monism à la Pauli and Jung. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19, 96-120.
|
[2]
|
Bolender, J. (2001). An argument for idealism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 37-61.
|
[3]
|
Bolte, A., & Goschke, T. (2005). On the speed of intuition: Intuitive judgments of semantic coherence under different response deadlines. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1248-1255. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193226
|
[4]
|
Carpenter, J. C. (2004). First sight: Part one, a model of psi and the mind. Journal of Parapsychology, 68, 217.
|
[5]
|
Carpenter, J. C. (2012). First sight. ESP and parapsychology in everyday life. New York: Rowman & Littlefield Pub. Inc..
|
[6]
|
Carpenter, J. C., Simmonds-Moore, C., Moore, S., & Carpenter, F. (2012). Extrasensory perception contributes to the formation of a preference: II. Confirmation of patterns found previously. Paper presented at the PA 55th Annual Convention, Durham, North Carolina August 9-12.
|
[7]
|
Clarke, C. J. S. (1995). The nonlocality of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 231-240.
|
[8]
|
Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1188595
|
[9]
|
Dunne, B. J., & Jahn, R. G. (2003). Information and uncertainty in remote perception research. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 17, 207-241.
|
[10]
|
Dossey, L. (1989). Recovering the soul. New York, NY: Bantam.
|
[11]
|
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709
|
[12]
|
Epstein, S. (2008). Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In H. Plessner, C. Betsch, & T. Betsch (Eds.), Intuition in judgment and decision making (pp. 23-37). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
|
[13]
|
Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
|
[14]
|
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223-241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
|
[15]
|
Fach, W., Atmanspacher, H., Landolt, K., Wyss, T., & Rossler, W. (2013). A comparative study of exceptional experiences of clients seeking advice and of subjects in an ordinary population. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 65. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00065
|
[16]
|
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148-163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.148
|
[17]
|
Friese, M., Wanke, M., & Plessner, H. (2006). Implicit consumer preferences and their influence on product choice. Psychology & Marketing, 23, 727-740. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20126
|
[18]
|
Greifeneder, R., Bless, H., & Pham, M. T. (2011). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 107-141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868310367640
|
[19]
|
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697-720. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
|
[20]
|
Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118, 97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020762
|
[21]
|
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
|
[22]
|
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgement. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49-81). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
|
[23]
|
Martens, U., Ansorge, U., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Controlling the unconscious attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuomotor processes differentially. Psychological Science, 22, 282-291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610397056
|
[24]
|
Mossbridge, J., Tressoldi, P., & Utts, J. (2012). Predictive physiological anticipation preceding seemingly unpredictable stimuli: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 390. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00390
|
[25]
|
Pihlstrom, S. (2008). Panpsychism—A neglected jamesian alternative? Journal of Philosophical Research, 32, 319-347. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/jpr20073238
|
[26]
|
Sherwood, S. J., & Roe, C. A. (2003). A review of dream ESP studies conducted since the maimonides dream ESP program. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 85-109.
|
[27]
|
Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014212
|
[28]
|
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Elrbaum.
|
[29]
|
Staune, J. (2013). Towards non-physical realism. In A. Suarez, & P. Adams (Eds.), Is science compatible with free will? (pp. 209-224). New York: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5212-6_14
|
[30]
|
Storm, L., Tressoldi, P. E., & Di Risio, L. (2010). Meta-analyses of free-response studies 1992-2008, assessing the noise reduction model in parapsychology. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 491-494. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019840
|
[31]
|
Storm, L., Tressoldi, P. E., & Di Risio, L. (2012). Meta-analysis of ESP studies, 1987-2010: Assessing the success of the forced-choice design in parapsychology. Journal of Parapsychology, 72, 243-273.
|
[32]
|
Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdsma, A., van Baaren, R. B., & Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Social Cognition, 29, 738-762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.738
|
[33]
|
Strick, M., van Noorden, T. H., Ritskes, R. R., de Ruiter, J. R., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Zen meditation and access to information in the unconscious. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1476-1481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.010
|
[34]
|
Tamietto, M. et al. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. PNAS, 106, 17661-17666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908994106
|
[35]
|
Tressoldi, P. E. (2011). Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence: The case of non-local perception, a classical and Bayesian review of evidences. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00117
|
[36]
|
Tressoldi, P., & Khrennikov, A. (2012). Remote state preparation of mental information: A theoretical model and a summary of experimental evidence. Neuro Quantology, 10, 394-402.
|
[37]
|
Tressoldi, P. E., Rabeyron, T., Duggan, M., & Bem, D. (submitted). Feeling the future: A meta-analysis of experiments on the anomalous anticipation of random future events.
|
[38]
|
Usher, M., Russo, Z., Weyers, M., Brauner, R., & Zakay, D. (2011). The impact of the mode of thought in complex decisions: Intuitive decisions are better. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 37. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00037
|
[39]
|
Van den Bussche, E., den Noortgate, W., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 452-477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015329
|
[40]
|
Velmans, M. (2012). Introduction to monist alternatives to physicalism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19, 9-10.
|