Does System 1 Process both Local and Nonlocal Information in Intuitive Judgment and Decision Making? Available Evidence and a Research Agenda Proposal

Abstract

This paper argues that System 1, (the mental processing system mainly involved in the processing of unconscious information) in contrast to System 2, (mainly involved in the processing of conscious information), processes not only local information conveyed by sensory organs, but also nonlocal ones, that is, those beyond the detection range of sensory organs. The striking similarities observed between the characteristics of local and nonlocal information processing by System 1, offer the possibility of using most of the experimental protocols used to investigate local information for the nonlocal information. Available evidence is presented and a research agenda is outlined that could raise fascinating questions and answer about the functioning of the human mind.

Share and Cite:

Tressoldi, P. (2013) Does System 1 Process both Local and Nonlocal Information in Intuitive Judgment and Decision Making? Available Evidence and a Research Agenda Proposal. Psychology, 4, 798-803. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.411114.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Atmanspacher, H. (2012). Dual-aspect monism à la Pauli and Jung. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19, 96-120.
[2] Bolender, J. (2001). An argument for idealism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 37-61.
[3] Bolte, A., & Goschke, T. (2005). On the speed of intuition: Intuitive judgments of semantic coherence under different response deadlines. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1248-1255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193226
[4] Carpenter, J. C. (2004). First sight: Part one, a model of psi and the mind. Journal of Parapsychology, 68, 217.
[5] Carpenter, J. C. (2012). First sight. ESP and parapsychology in everyday life. New York: Rowman & Littlefield Pub. Inc..
[6] Carpenter, J. C., Simmonds-Moore, C., Moore, S., & Carpenter, F. (2012). Extrasensory perception contributes to the formation of a preference: II. Confirmation of patterns found previously. Paper presented at the PA 55th Annual Convention, Durham, North Carolina August 9-12.
[7] Clarke, C. J. S. (1995). The nonlocality of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 231-240.
[8] Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1188595
[9] Dunne, B. J., & Jahn, R. G. (2003). Information and uncertainty in remote perception research. Journal of Scientific Exploration, 17, 207-241.
[10] Dossey, L. (1989). Recovering the soul. New York, NY: Bantam.
[11] Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709
[12] Epstein, S. (2008). Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In H. Plessner, C. Betsch, & T. Betsch (Eds.), Intuition in judgment and decision making (pp. 23-37). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[13] Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
[14] Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 223-241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
[15] Fach, W., Atmanspacher, H., Landolt, K., Wyss, T., & Rossler, W. (2013). A comparative study of exceptional experiences of clients seeking advice and of subjects in an ordinary population. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 65. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00065
[16] Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with relationship partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148-163.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.148
[17] Friese, M., Wanke, M., & Plessner, H. (2006). Implicit consumer preferences and their influence on product choice. Psychology & Marketing, 23, 727-740. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20126
[18] Greifeneder, R., Bless, H., & Pham, M. T. (2011). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 107-141.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868310367640
[19] Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697-720.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
[20] Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118, 97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020762
[21] Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
[22] Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgement. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49-81). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
[23] Martens, U., Ansorge, U., & Kiefer, M. (2011). Controlling the unconscious attentional task sets modulate subliminal semantic and visuomotor processes differentially. Psychological Science, 22, 282-291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610397056
[24] Mossbridge, J., Tressoldi, P., & Utts, J. (2012). Predictive physiological anticipation preceding seemingly unpredictable stimuli: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 390.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00390
[25] Pihlstrom, S. (2008). Panpsychism—A neglected jamesian alternative? Journal of Philosophical Research, 32, 319-347.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/jpr20073238
[26] Sherwood, S. J., & Roe, C. A. (2003). A review of dream ESP studies conducted since the maimonides dream ESP program. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 85-109.
[27] Sio, U. N., & Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 94-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014212
[28] Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Elrbaum.
[29] Staune, J. (2013). Towards non-physical realism. In A. Suarez, & P. Adams (Eds.), Is science compatible with free will? (pp. 209-224). New York: Springer.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-5212-6_14
[30] Storm, L., Tressoldi, P. E., & Di Risio, L. (2010). Meta-analyses of free-response studies 1992-2008, assessing the noise reduction model in parapsychology. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 491-494.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019840
[31] Storm, L., Tressoldi, P. E., & Di Risio, L. (2012). Meta-analysis of ESP studies, 1987-2010: Assessing the success of the forced-choice design in parapsychology. Journal of Parapsychology, 72, 243-273.
[32] Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdsma, A., van Baaren, R. B., & Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Social Cognition, 29, 738-762.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.6.738
[33] Strick, M., van Noorden, T. H., Ritskes, R. R., de Ruiter, J. R., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Zen meditation and access to information in the unconscious. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1476-1481.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.010
[34] Tamietto, M. et al. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. PNAS, 106, 17661-17666.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908994106
[35] Tressoldi, P. E. (2011). Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence: The case of non-local perception, a classical and Bayesian review of evidences. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 117.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00117
[36] Tressoldi, P., & Khrennikov, A. (2012). Remote state preparation of mental information: A theoretical model and a summary of experimental evidence. Neuro Quantology, 10, 394-402.
[37] Tressoldi, P. E., Rabeyron, T., Duggan, M., & Bem, D. (submitted). Feeling the future: A meta-analysis of experiments on the anomalous anticipation of random future events.
[38] Usher, M., Russo, Z., Weyers, M., Brauner, R., & Zakay, D. (2011). The impact of the mode of thought in complex decisions: Intuitive decisions are better. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 37.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00037
[39] Van den Bussche, E., den Noortgate, W., & Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 452-477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015329
[40] Velmans, M. (2012). Introduction to monist alternatives to physicalism. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 19, 9-10.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.