[1]
|
De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313, 684-687. doi:10.1126/science.1128356
|
[2]
|
Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Kramer, J., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., Kenning, P., & Ringelstein, E. B. (2005). Evidence for a neural correlate of a framing effect: Bias-specific activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during credibility judgments. Brain Research Bulletin, 67, 413-421. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.017
|
[3]
|
Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Pieper, A., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., Kenning, P., & Ringelstein, E. B. (2007). Anterior cingulate reflects susceptibility to framing during attractiveness evaluation. Neuroreport, 18, 1119-1123. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282202c61
|
[4]
|
Druckman, J. N. (2001a). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23, 225-256.
|
[5]
|
Druckman, J. N. (2001b). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of Politics, 63, 1041-1066.
|
[6]
|
Frisch, D. (1993). Reasons for framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 399-429.
doi:10.1006/obhd.1993.1017
|
[7]
|
Gonzalez, C., Dana, J., Koshino, H., & Just, M. (2005). The framing effect and risky decisions: Examining cognitive functions with fMRI. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26, 1-20.
doi:10.1016/j.joep.2004.08.004
|
[8]
|
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory—An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263-291.
doi:10.2307/1914185
|
[9]
|
Kuo, W. J., Sjostrom, T., Chen, Y. P., Wang, Y. H., & Huang, C. Y. (2009). Intuition and deliberation: Two systems for strategizing in the brain. Science, 324, 519-522. doi:10.1126/science.1165598
|
[10]
|
Lakshminarayanan, V. R., Chen, M. K., & Santos, L. R. (2011). The evolution of decision-making under risk: Framing effects in monkey risk preferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 689-693. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.011
|
[11]
|
Levin, I. P., Gaeth, G. J., Schreiber, J., & Lauriola, M. (2002). A new look at framing effects: Distribution of effect sizes, individual differences, and independence of types of effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 411-429.
doi:10.1006/obhd.2001.2983
|
[12]
|
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149-188. doi:10.1006/obhd.1998.2804
|
[13]
|
Ma, Q. G., Feng, Y. D., Xu, Q., Bian, J., & Tang, H. X. (2012). Brain potentials associated with the outcome processing in framing effects. Neuroscience Letters, 528, 110-113.
doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.016
|
[14]
|
Marsh, B., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Framing effects and risky decisions in starlings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 3352-3355.
doi:10.1073/pnas.042491999
|
[15]
|
Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19, 221-246.
doi:10.1023/A:1024834831093
|
[16]
|
Reyna, V. F., & Ellis, S. C. (1994). Fuzzy-trace theory and framing effects in children’s risky decision-making. Psychological Science, 5, 275-279. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00625.x
|
[17]
|
Smith, S. M., & Levin, I. P. (1996). Need for cognition and choice framing effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 283-290.
doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199612)9:4<283::AID-BDM241>3.0.CO;2-7
|
[18]
|
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453-458.
|
[19]
|
Zhang, F., Zeng, J., & Zhang, Q. (2010). Framing effect: Affective heuristics. Psychological Science (in China), 33, 1375-1380.
|