[1]
|
Abrams, R. L., Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 100-106.
doi:10.3758/BF03196262
|
[2]
|
Alford, R. L., & Strother, J. B. (1990). Attitudes of native and non-native speakers toward selected regional accents of US English. TESOL Quarterly, 3, 479-495. doi:10.2307/3587231
|
[3]
|
Allen, J. J., Iacono, W. G., & Danielson, K. D. (1992). The identification of concealed memories using the event-related brain potential and implicit behavioral measures: A methodology for prediction in the face of individual differences. Psychophysiology, 29, 504-522.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02024.x
|
[4]
|
Ansorge, U., Francis, G., Herzog, M. H., & ??men, H. (2007). Visual masking and the dynamics of human perception, cognition, and consciousness. A century of progress, a contemporary synthesis, and future directions advances in cognitive psychology, 3, 1-2, 1-8.
|
[5]
|
Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B, & Garozzo, R. (1979). Visual evoked potentials and affective ratings of semantic stimuli. In H. Begleiter (Ed.), Evoked brain potentials and behaviour (pp. 127-141). New York: Plenum Press.
|
[6]
|
Baker, S. J., Chrzan, G. J., Park, C. N., & Saunders, J. H. (1986). Behavioral effects of 0% and 0.05% blood alcohol in male volunteers. Neurobehavioral Toxicology & Teratology, 8, 77-81.
|
[7]
|
Ball, P. (1983). Stereotypes of Anglo-Saxon and Non-Anglo-Saxon accents: Some exploratory Australian studies with the matched guise technique. Language Sciences, 5, 163-183.
doi:10.1016/S0388-0001(83)80021-7
|
[8]
|
Bazan, A., Shevrin, H., Brakel, L. A. W., & Snodgrass, M. (2007). Motivations and emotions contribute to a-rational unconscious dynamics: Evidence and conceptual clarification. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 43, 1104.
|
[9]
|
Bayard, D. (1990). God help us if we all sound like this: Attitudes to New Zealand and other English accents. In A. Bell, & J. Holmes (Eds.), New Zealand ways of speaking English (pp. 67-96). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd.
|
[10]
|
Benschop, R. (1998). What is a tachistoscope? Historical explorations of an instrument. Science in Context, 11, 23-50.
doi:10.1017/S0269889700002908
|
[11]
|
Beznosjuk, E. V., & Smirnov, I. V. (1990). Психокорреция, психопрофилактика, психотерапия//Медицинская психология и психогигиена. М.1, ММИ, 1990. С. 21. [Psycho-correction, psycho-prophylactic, psychotherapy.] Medical Psychology and Psycho-Hygiene, M.1, MMI, 21.
|
[12]
|
Bunce, S. C., Bernat, E., Wong, P. S., & Shevrin, H. (1999). Further evidence for unconscious learning: Preliminary support for the conditioning of facial EMG to subliminal stimuli. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33, 341-347. doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(99)00003-5
|
[13]
|
Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401-423. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.401
|
[14]
|
Cheshire, J. (1982). Linguistic variation and social function. In S. Romaine (Ed.), Sociolinguistic variation in speech communities (pp. 153-166). London: Edward Arnold.
|
[15]
|
Cobb, S. R., Cole, C., & Rainey, M. T. (1978). The effect of exposure time upon perceived size. British Jounal Physiology Ophtalmology, 32, 94-96.
|
[16]
|
Costrica, V. G., Tkachenko, A. E., & Malcev, S. I. (1998). Use of a computer psycho-semantic analysis allows exposure of persons who are prone to alcohol and drug abuse and asocial behavior. Proceedings of TRTU, 4, 54-55.
|
[17]
|
Damphousse, K. R., Pointon, L., Upchurch, D., & Moore, R. K. (2007). Assessing the Validity of voice stress analysis tools in a jail setting. Report submitted to the US Department of Justice.
http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/219031.pdf
|
[18]
|
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 204-211.
|
[19]
|
Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., & Smith, P. K. (2005). The power of the subliminal: Subliminal perception and possible applications. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconsciousness (pp. 77-106). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
|
[20]
|
Dixon, N. (1971). Subliminal perception: The nature of controversy. London: McGraw-Hill.
|
[21]
|
Dixon, N. (1981). Preconscious processing. New York: John Wiley.
|
[22]
|
El-Dash, L., & Tucker, R. (1975). Subjective reactions to various speech styles in Egypt. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 6, 33-54. doi:10.1515/ijsl.1975.6.33
|
[23]
|
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709
|
[24]
|
Eriksson, A., & Lacerda, F. (2007). Charlatanry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously. The International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law, 14, 169-193.
|
[25]
|
Esteves, F., Parra, C., Dimberg, U., & Ohman, A. (1994). Nonconscious associative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. Psychophysiology, 31, 375-385.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02446.x
|
[26]
|
Gaies, S. J., & Beebe, J. D. (1991). The matched-guise technique for measuring attitudes and their implications for language education: A critical assessment. In E. Sadtano (Ed.), Language acquisition and the second/foreign language classroom. Anthology Series 28. Singapore: SEAMEO Regional Language Centre.
|
[27]
|
Gallois, C., & Callan, V. J. (1981). Personality impressions elicited by accented English speech. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 12, 247-359. doi:10.1177/0022022181123006
|
[28]
|
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. American Psychologist, 47, 766-779.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.47.6.766
|
[29]
|
Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., & Schuh, E. S. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (“subliminal”) stimuli: Dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 22-42. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.1.22
|
[30]
|
Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699-1702. doi:10.1126/science.273.5282.1699
|
[31]
|
Greenwald, A. G., Abrams, R. L., Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). Long-term semantic memory versus contextual memory in unconscious number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 235-247.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.235
|
[32]
|
Grubin, D. J. (2010) The polygraph and forensic psychiatry. American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 38, 446-451.
|
[33]
|
Heise, D. R. (2010) Surveying cultures: Discovering shared conceptions and sentiments. John Wiley and Sons Ltd.
|
[34]
|
Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1-23.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X00021269
|
[35]
|
Ioffe, S., Yesin, S. V., Afanasjev, B. G., & Nezhdanov, I. K. (2007a). Psychosemantic diagnosis of alcoholic dependencies tested at the unconscious level in military personnel with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Polygraph Journal, 2, 57-69.
|
[36]
|
Ioffe, S., Yesin, S. V., Afanasjev, B. G., & Nezhdanov, I. K. (2007b). The role of unconscious effects during the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder with alcohol dependencies in military personnel. Polygraph Journal, 3, 133-148.
|
[37]
|
Ioffe, S., & Konobeevsky, M. (2008). Psycho-semantic spheres of the personality among correctional facility employees. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 23, 23-34.
doi:10.1007/s11896-008-9018-7
|
[38]
|
Ioffe, S., & Yesin, S. (2010). Psyche from within: Three clinical case studies. Psychology, 1, 282-294. doi:10.4236/psych.2010.14037
|
[39]
|
Ioffe, S., & Yesin, S. (2011). Controlled reactivation of psycho-semantic space by unconscious (subthreshold) stimuli. Psychology, 2, 181-186. doi:10.4236/psych.2011.23029
|
[40]
|
Jones, E. E., & Sigall, H. (1971). The bogus pipeline: A new paradigm for measuring affect and attitude. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 349-364. doi:10.1037/h0031617
|
[41]
|
Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.
|
[42]
|
Kelly, G. A. (1970). Behaviour is an experiment. Perspectives in personal construct theory. London: Academic Press.
|
[43]
|
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445-1452. doi:10.1126/science.3629249
|
[44]
|
Kleiner, M. (2002). Physiological detection of deception in psychological perspectives: A theoretical proposal, in Handbook of Polygraph Testing. London: Academic Press.
|
[45]
|
Knight, D. C., Nguyen, H. T., & Bandettini, P. A. (2003). Expression of conditional fear with and without awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 15280-15283.
doi:10.1073/pnas.2535780100
|
[46]
|
Kostandov, E. A. (1977) Восприятие и эмоции. М., 1977. 316 с. [Perceptions and emotions] M. 716.
|
[47]
|
Kostandov, E. A. (1983). Функциональная асимметрия полушарий мозга и неосознаваемое восприятие. М., 1983. 246 с. [Functional asymmetry of cerebral hemispheres and unconscious perception] M. 246.
|
[48]
|
Kostandov, E. A. (1984). Сознание и бессознательное как проблема физиологии высшей нервной деятельности человека //Журнал высшей нервной деятельности. 1984, т.34, N 3, С.401-411. [Consciousness and unconscious as a physiology of higher neural activity in human.] Journal of Higher Neural Activity, 3, 401-411.
|
[49]
|
Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557-558.
doi:10.1126/science.7352271
|
[50]
|
Lang, S. F., Nelson, C. A., & Collins, P. F. (1990). Event-related potentials to emotional and neutral stimuli. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 946-958.
doi:10.1080/01688639008401033
|
[51]
|
Leontiev, V. O. (2002). Классификация эмоций. 2002.Одесса: Изд-во Инновационно-ипотечного центра, [Classification of the emotions] Odessa: Innovation centre.
http://emoatlas.narod.ru/book1.html
|
[52]
|
Lippold, O. C. (1970). Oscillation in the stretch reflex arc and the origin of the rhythmical, 8 - 12 c/s component of physiological tremor. The Journal of Physiology, 206, 359-382.
|
[53]
|
Lippold, O. C. (1971). Physiological tremor. Scientific American, 224, 65-73. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0371-65
|
[54]
|
Luriya, A. R., & Vinogradova, O. S. (1971). Объективное исследование динамики семантических систем//Семантическая структура слова. М.: Наука, 1971, С. 27-63. Objective research of the dynamics of the semantic systems.] In Semantic structure of the word. M. Science, 27-63.
|
[55]
|
Macmillan, N. (1986). The psychophysics of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 38-39.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X00021427
|
[56]
|
National Research Council (2003). The polygraph and lie detection. Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph. Washington DC: The National Academies Press.
|
[57]
|
Ohman, A., & Soares, J. F. (1994). “Unconscious anxiety”: Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231-240. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103.2.231
|
[58]
|
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: Illinois Press.
|
[59]
|
Osgood, C. E. (1959). Representational model and relevant research methods. In I. Pool (Ed.), Trends in content analysis (pp. 33-38). Urbana, IL: Illinois Press.
|
[60]
|
Osgood, C. E. (1967). Semantic differential technique in the comparative study of cultures. In L. A. Jakobovite, & M. S. Miron (Eds.), Readings in the psychology of language (pp. 111-199). Englewood Cliffs, Prentic-Hall.
|
[61]
|
Osgood, C. E. (1976). Focus of meaning. Mouton.
|
[62]
|
Pessiglione, M., Petrovic, P., Daunizeau, J., Palminteri, S., Dolan, R. J., Frith, C. D. (2008) Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain neuron. 59, 561-567.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.005
|
[63]
|
Ramachandran, V. S., & Cobb, S. (1995). Visual attention modulates metacontrast masking. Nature, 373, 21.
|
[64]
|
Reingold, E., & Merikle, P. (1988). Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness. Perception and Psychophysics, 44, 563-575. doi:10.3758/BF03207490
|
[65]
|
Reingold, E., & Merikle, P. (1990). On the inter-relatedness of theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9-28. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1990.tb00150.x
|
[66]
|
Renshaw, S. (1945). The visual perception and reproduction of forms by tachistoscopic methods. Journal of Psychology, 20, 217-232.
doi:10.1080/00223980.1945.9917254
|
[67]
|
Senter, S., Weatherman, D., Krapohl, D., & Horvath, F. (2010). Psychological set or differential salience: A proposal for reconciling theory and terminology in polygraph testing. Polygraph, 39, 109-117.
|
[68]
|
Shevrin, H, & Fritzler, D. (1968). Visual evoked response correlates of unconscious mental processes. Science, 161, 295-298.
doi:10.1126/science.161.3838.295
|
[69]
|
Shevrin, H., Smith, W., & Fritzler, D. (1969). Repressiveness as a factor in the subliminal activation of brain and verbal responses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 149, 261-269.
doi:10.1097/00005053-196909000-00003
|
[70]
|
Shevrin, H. (1973). Brain wave correlates of subliminal stimulation, unconscious attention, primay- and secondary-process thinking, and repressiveness. Psychological Issues Monograph 30, Psychoanalytic Research, 8, 56-87.
|
[71]
|
Shevrin, H., & Dickman, S. (1980). The psychological unconscious, a necessary assumption for all psychological theory? American Psychologist, 35, 421-435. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.35.5.421
|
[72]
|
Shevrin, H., Williams, W. J., Marshall, R. E., Hertel, R. K., Bond, J. A., & Brakel, L. A. (1992). Event-related potential indicators of the dynamic unconscious. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 340-366.
doi:10.1016/1053-8100(92)90068-L
|
[73]
|
Shevrin, H., Bond, J. A., Brakel, L. A. W., Hertel, R. K., & Williams, W. J. (1996). Conscious and unconscious processes: Psychodynamic, cognitive, and neurophysioloical nonvergences. New York: Guilford Press.
|
[74]
|
Smirnov, I., Beznosjuk, E. & Zhuravlyov, A. (1995). Психотехнологии: компьютерный психосемантический анализ и психокоррекция на неосознаваемом уровне. - М., 1995. С. 98 – 103. [Psychotechnologies: Computer psychosemantic analysis and psychocorrection at unconscious level.] M. “Culture”, 98-103.
|
[75]
|
Snodgrass, J. M. (2000). Unconscious perception: Theory, method, and evidence. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
|
[76]
|
Tjuvina, N. A., & Krivtsov, K. J. U. (2006) Применение метода компьютерного психосемантического анализа для ранней диагностики возможного рецидива у больных параноидной формой шизофрении в период формирования медикаментозной ремиссии. Журнал неврологии и психиатрии им. С. С. Корсакова. 106 (11) 50-53. [Application of a method of the computer psycho-semantic analysis for early diagnostics of possible relapse in the patients with the paranoid form of a schizophrenia during of medicamentous remission.] Journal
|
[77]
|
Waller, G., & Mijatovich, S. (1998) Preconscious processing of threat cues: Impact on eating among women with unhealthy eating attitudes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 24, 83-89.
doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199807)24:1<83::AID-EAT7>3.0.CO;2-X
|
[78]
|
Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of American Statistical Association, 58, 236-244.
doi:10.2307/2282967
|
[79]
|
Wedding, D., & Stalans, L. (1985) Hemispheric differences in the perception of positive and negative faces. International Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 277-281. doi:10.3109/00207458509149773
|
[80]
|
Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121-135.
doi:10.1177/0146167204271309
|
[81]
|
Wong, P. S., Shevrin, H., & Williams, W. J. (1994). Conscious and nonconscious processes: An ERP index of an anticipatory response in a conditioning paradigm using visually masked stimuli. Psychopsysiology, 31, 87-101. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01028.x
|
[82]
|
Wong, P. S., Bernat, E., Bunce, S. C., & Shevrin, H. (1997). Brain indices of nonconscious associative learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 519-544. doi:10.1006/ccog.1997.0322
|
[83]
|
Zayas, V., Greenwald, A. G., & Osterhout, L. (2011). Unintentional covert motor activations predict behavioral effects: Multilevel modeling of trial-level electrophysiological motor activations. Psychophysiology, 48, 208-217. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01055.x
|
[84]
|
Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye Movements and Vision. New York: Plenum Press.
|
[85]
|
Disclaimer: The views and opinions expressed in the article are those of the author (or authors) and not the Department of Defense.
|