Developing and Validating a Japanese Version of the Multidimensional Attitude toward Ambiguity Scale (MAAS) ()
1. Introduction
The spread of the COVID-19 pandemic and the ambiguity and uncertainty surrounding the spread of the virus via misinformation and confusing, contradictory government policies have had a great impact on people’s mental well-being and their attitudes toward world events (e.g., Koffman et al., 2020). It has demonstrated the need to examine the psychological mechanisms behind people’s responses to ambiguity and uncertainty (Salvi et al., 2021). Below, we examine how these mechanisms have been studied in the literature to give some background for this study’s development and validation of a Japanese version of the Multidimensional Attitude toward Ambiguity Scale (MAAS).
1.1. Ambiguity Tolerance
Since Frenkel-Brunswik (1949) first coined the concept of ambiguity tolerance, the concept has been studied and applied in various fields, including cognitive psychology, clinical psychology, medicine, and organizational behavior (Furnham & Marks, 2013). Research on ambiguity tolerance has focused on people’s negative attitudes toward ambiguous situations (Nishimura, 2007) by measuring aspects such as their intolerance of uncertainty—defined as “the tendency to perceive ambiguous situations as a source of threat” (Budner, 1962: p. 29). Therefore, studies of ambiguity tolerance have also examined the relationship between low levels of ambiguity tolerance and social anxiety (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009), worry (Buhr & Dugas, 2006), and stress evaluation (Iannello et al., 2017; Masuda, 1998). Other scholars have not framed ambiguity tolerance as perception of a threat; McLain (1993) suggested that it represents “a range, from rejection to attraction, of reactions to stimuli perceived as unfamiliar, complex, dynamically uncertain, or subject to multiple conflicting interpretations” (p. 184). Following McLain, some researchers have emphasized the relationship between ambiguity tolerance and people’s attraction to or desire for ambiguity and have found that high ambiguity tolerance is robustly correlated with openness to experience (Jach & Smillie, 2019), preferences for certain genres of art (Furnham & Avison, 1997; Swami et al., 2010), and is associated with sensation seeking (McLain, 2009). These studies have largely used subjective self-reporting scales. By 2013, roughly eight such scales had been developed—however, their psychometric quality is rather low (Furnham & Marks, 2013).
1.2. Scales to Measure Ambiguity Tolerance
Lauriola et al. (2016) proposed the Multidimensional Attitude toward Ambiguity Scale (MAAS) in response to these aforementioned problems. The MAAS is a high quality scale derived from a factor analysis of the hierarchical structure of 133 items from seven other ambiguity tolerance scales. It assesses people’s multidimensional attitudes toward ambiguity via three subscales: their discomfort with ambiguity (DA), moral absolutism/splitting (MA), and need for complexity and novelty (NC).
The DA represents a negative attitude toward or intolerance of ambiguity. High scores on this subscale are positively related to social anxiety (Boelen & Reijntjes, 2009), stress appraisal (Iannello et al., 2017), negative affect (Bardi et al., 2009; Iannello et al., 2020), and neurotic tendencies regarding the Big Five personality traits (Jach & Smillie, 2019; Matthews et al., 2018). The MA refers to the attitude of judging things in a binary, “black and white” manner. People with high MA scores tend to try to eliminate ambiguity. The MA is related to the need for cognitive completion, or a “desire for definite knowledge on some issue and the eschewal of confusion and ambiguity” (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996: p. 278). van Zyl’s (2020) study examined the relationship between high MA scores and cognitive tasks. This study found that MA (but not DA or NC) negatively predicted study participants’ performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) and belief-bias syllogisms (BB), and that participants with higher MA scores performed worse on these tasks (van Zyl, 2020). In contrast to DA and MA, NC refers to people’s positive attitudes toward complexity, novelty, and ambiguity (Budner, 1962). High NC scores are associated with openness and extraversion (Jach & Smillie, 2019) and are also thought to be related to positive emotions (Bardi et al., 2009), a tendency toward mindfulness (Le et al., 2012), and a high tolerance of ambiguity.
By capturing people’s multidimensional attitudes toward ambiguity, the MAAS helps researchers examine people’s attitudes toward ambiguity from multiple perspectives rather than merely assessing whether people view ambiguity as positive or negative. These three subscales also reflect the affective (DA), cognitive (MA), and epistemic (NC) aspects of people’s attitudes toward ambiguity (Lauriola et al., 2016), and have been applied in this manner in the literature (Childers et al., 2020 for NC; Matthews et al., 2018 for DA; Salvi et al., 2021 for MA). Although the MAAS is valid and reliable, there is no Japanese version of the MAAS available. This study, thus, fills a practical and theoretical gap by developing and validating a Japanese version of the MAAS.
1.3. Limitations of Other Japanese Scales Measuring Ambiguity Tolerance
Other ambiguity tolerance scales are already used in Japan. For instance, researchers created the Ambiguity Tolerance Scale IV (ATS-IV) based on the items of foreign versions scales developed by Norton (1975) and created a one-dimensional factor structure (Imagawa, 1981). However, Nishimura and Kitayama (2001) found that this scale had not one but two dimensions, that is, the results of factor analysis were not consistent. In addition, it was translated nearly 40 years ago, therefore, the wording of the items needs to be updated, and with its 44 items it is difficult and time-consuming to fill out. Although an original Japanese version of the scale exists (Nishimura, 2007; Tomono & Hashimoto, 2005), it is difficult to make cross-cultural comparisons between scales that have different items and factor structures. Cross-cultural comparisons are important because attitudes toward ambiguity might be culturally determined and include differences between Eastern and Western populations (Bottesi et al., 2016; Gelfand et al., 2011; Lauriola et al., 2016; Spector et al., 2001). Thus, by creating a Japanese version of the MAAS—a scale which is already widely used overseas—the authors hope to facilitate cross-cultural comparisons and research. The scale has already been translated into Italian (Lauriola et al., 2016) and Swedish (Forsberg et al., 2019), and translations into other languages are in progress.
1.4. Purpose of This Study
This study aims to create a Japanese version of the MAAS devised by Lauriola et al. (2016) and verify its reliability and validity. First, we will check whether the back-translated Japanese version of the MAAS has the same factor structure as the original. Second, we will conduct a survey using our new MAAS to confirm its re-test reliability and compare its construct validity with other scales. Throughout, we place emphasis on the strength of the scale to make it more suitable for cross-cultural comparison.
2. Method and Measures
This study was conducted after ethical review by the Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University (Acceptance Number: CPE-379).
2.1. Participants
We recruited 400 study participants through Crowdworks (https://crowdworks.jp/). Of these, we analyzed the survey responses of 385 participants (159 females and 226 males, Mage = 38.63, SDage = 10.48)—we arrived at this number by excluding participants with the same IP address, extremely short response times (less than five minutes), and those who failed to answer the attention check correctly. To assess the reliability of our Japanese MAAS, we conducted a re-test by asking our study participants to answer the same questionnaire one week later. After linking the data between the two test days, we found that we had a total 347 participants (147 females and 200 males, Mage = 39.07, SDage = 10.58).
2.2. Procedure
We made the questionnaire using Qualtrics (https://www.qualtrics.com/) and the participants entered responses to the questionnaire on Crowdworks. The questionnaire began by asking for the participants’ demographic data (gender, age, nationality, level of education attained, etc.), followed by the 30-item Japanese version of the MAAS (MAAS-J). After that, to examine construct validity, participants responded to seven scales known to be associated with attitudes toward ambiguity in a randomized order (see scales, below). The questionnaire items for these questionnaires were also randomized. It took about 10 - 15 minutes to answer the questionnaires and participants were paid 150 yen for their time.
2.3. Multidimensional Attitude toward Ambiguity Scale
The authors of this paper translated the MAAS into Japanese with the permission of the original authors. We tried to be faithful to the original version’s intent. We then used a translation service (NAI Inc.; https://www.nai.co.jp/) to back-translate a provisional Japanese version of the MAAS. Finally, we had the original authors confirm that the back-translated version of the MAAS-J was equivalent to the original English version.
Then, after several preliminary surveys of 100 - 150 participants each, we made a minor correction to the items. These surveys found that the original 30-item MAAS-J had low factor loadings and that many items were dropped. They also found that a shortened, 21-item version of the MAAS-J had better factor integration. Therefore, we decided to use this shortened version in our main study (for the model fit, items, and factor analysis of the 30-item version of the MAAS-J, please refer to Supplementary Table 1 and Table 2). Like the MAAS, the MAAS-J consists of three factors—DA, MA, and NC. Survey respondents were asked to rate their attitudes toward various ambiguous situations on a seven-point Likert-type scale, where responses ranged from “completely disagree” to “strongly agree”. The 21-item version in Japanese can be found in the supplementary file.
2.4. Short Form of the Big Five Scale
This scale is a short version of the Big Five Scale (BFS) developed by Wada (1996). The BFS was developed from a translation of the Adjective Check List (ACL) developed by Gough and Heilbrun (1965). The short version of the BFS, which includes five items related to neuroticism, five items related to extraversion, and six items related to openness, was used. This version has been confirmed to have factorial validity, internal consistency, and comorbid validity (Namikawa et al., 2012). Previous studies have found that DA is positively correlated with neuroticism and negatively correlated with extraversion and openness, MA negatively correlated with openness, and NC positively correlated with extraversion and openness (Lauriola et al., 2016). We expect to find similar correlations in the present study.
2.5. Need for Closure Scale
The Need for Closure Scale (NFC) is used to measure people’s need for cognitive closure. The original version of this scale, developed by Webster and Kruglanski (1994), had 41 items with five factors. In this study, we used the Japanese version developed by Suzuki and Sakurai (2003). The Japanese version of the NFC has 20 items and a three-factor structure (Neuberg et al., 1997). The three factors are decisiveness, preference for order (PFO), and preference for predictability (PFP). The internal consistency, re-test reliability, and construct validity of the Japanese version of the NFC have all been confirmed. Participants were asked to rate their agreement with each item on a seven-point Likert-type scale with responses ranging from “completely disagree” to “strongly agree”. The need for cognitive closure is considered to be similar to ambiguity tolerance and intolerance of
![]()
Table 1. Fit indices of the confirmatory factor analysis for the 21-item MAAS-J.
Note. Chisq = chi square; GFI = goodness of fit index; AGFI = adjusted GFI; NFI = normed fit index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike’s information criterion.
![]()
Table 2. Result of the confirmatory factor analysis for the 21-item MAAS-J.
uncertainty and has been correlated with scales that measure NFC (Gärtner et al., 2020). Among the three factors, PFO and PFP are correlated with a tendency toward a lack of ambiguity and decisiveness is correlated with making quick, confident decisions, and not being perturbed by ambiguity. We expect to find MA and DA to be positively correlated with PFO and PFP and negatively correlated with decisiveness and expect to find the opposite for NC.
2.6. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) was developed by Watson et al. (1988) to measure positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). A Japanese version of the PANAS was developed by Sato and Yasuda (2001). The original version consisted of 20 items with 10 items each; the Japanese version contains only 16 items with eight items each by deleting items which had low factor loadings. The validity of this scale has been confirmed using the internal consistency and imagery methods. Participants responded to each item using a six-point Likert-type scale with answers ranging from “not true at all” to “very true”. Previous studies have found that intolerance of ambiguity is negatively correlated with PA and positively correlated with NA (Bardi et al., 2009; Iannello et al., 2020). We expect to find DA to be positively correlated with NA and NC to be positively correlated with PA.
2.7. State Trait Anxiety Inventory
The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) was developed by Spielberger et al. (1970). It consists of 20 items for state anxiety and 20 items for trait anxiety. A Japanese version of the STAI was developed by Nakazato and Mizuguchi (1982), and they confirmed the internal consistency, re-test reliability, and construct validity of this scale have been confirmed by previous studies. Participants were asked to answer each item using a four-point Likert-type scale with answers ranging from “almost never” to “frequently”. Bardi et al.’s (2009) study found that there is a positive correlation between intolerance of ambiguity and trait anxiety, therefore, we expect to find a positive correlation between trait anxiety and DA and a negative correlation between trait anxiety and NC.
2.8. Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire
The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ), developed by Baer et al. (2006), is a 39-item scale used to multidimensionally assess trait mindfulness. A Japanese version of this scale was developed by Sugiura et al. (2012), and they have the internal consistency and construct validity of this scale. The Japanese version consists of 39 items with five factors: observing, describing, acting with awareness (AWA), non-judging (NJ), and non-reactivity (NR). Participants were asked to answer each item on a five-point Likert-type scale with answers ranging from “never” to “always”. Previous studies have found correlations between high ambiguity tolerance and mindfulness (Fulton, 2016; Le et al., 2012; Robinson, 2019). We expect to find negative correlations between mindfulness and DA and positive correlations between mindfulness and NC. Since previous studies have not examined the correlation between the multidimensional measures of mindfulness and attitudes toward ambiguity, we also explore the correlation between each subscale to facilitate comparative research. We expect MA to be negatively correlated with NJ.
2.9. Satisfaction with Life Scale
The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS), developed by Diener et al. (1985), is a five-item scale used to measure subjective life satisfaction. A Japanese version of the SWLS was developed by Sumino (1994), and they confirmed the internal consistency, re-test reliability, and construct validity of this scale. Participants answered each item using a seven-point Likert-type scale with answers ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. Since negative attitudes toward ambiguity are known to be associated with low life satisfaction (Bardi et al., 2009; Iannello et al., 2020), we expect a negative correlation between life satisfaction and DA and a positive correlation between life satisfaction and NC.
2.10. Minimalist Well-Being Scale
The Minimalist Well-Being Scale (MWBS), developed by Kan et al. (2009), is a 12-item scale used to measure a minimalist (i.e., non-material or consumption-driven) sense of well-being. The original version of the scale had a two-factor structure—the factors were peaceful disengagement (PD) and gratitude. However, the Japanese version of the scale developed by Horike and Ohshima (2015) has a four-factor structure. The factor structures are not consistent, so the total score of the entire scale is used here. The internal consistency and criterion-related validity of this scale have been confirmed by previous research (Kan et al., 2009). Respondents were asked to answer each item using a seven-point Likert-type scale with answers ranging from “I strongly disagree” to “I strongly agree”. We hope to expand our knowledge of the interaction between well-being and attitudes toward ambiguity by measuring the relationship between attitudes toward ambiguity and life satisfaction. We expect to find a correlation between ambiguity tolerance and life satisfaction.
3. Results
3.1. Examination of Factor Structure
When devising the original MAAS, Lauriola et al. (2016) examined a bi-factor model including a general factor of attitude toward ambiguity in addition to the three-factor structure of DA, MA, and NC. Following the original version, we conducted both confirmatory factor analysis using only three factors and confirmatory factor analysis including general factors for the MAAS-J. The results showed that the model fit well (Table1). Tableshows the contents and factor loadings of the 21-item version of the MAAS-J. Only item 14 had a factor loading below 0.40. We also examined the model fit and factor loadings for the 20 items that were deleted from the original MAAS (see Supplementary TableS1 and TableS2), but there was no significant difference in the values. Since we believe that a scale which is faithful to the original version is more useful for cross-cultural comparisons, we analyzed the 21-item MAAS-J because it is considered to have the same structure as the original version.
3.2. Examination of Reliability and Descriptive Statistics
The results of the factor analysis indicated that the MAAS-J has a three-factor structure consistent with that of the original version, therefore, the reliability and the descriptive statistics of each factor were examined. We calculated the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the internal consistency of each subscale of the MAAS-J. We found sufficient or good internal consistency was found for each subscale (DA: α = 0.73; MA: α = 0.75; NC: α = 0.84), as shown in Table 3. In addition, the results of the analysis of the correlations among the subscales for the re-test reliability, conducted after a one-week interval, showed sufficient or good re-test reliability (DA:r = 0.69, p < 0.01; MA: r = 0.73, p < 0.01; NC: r = 0.83, p < 0.01). We also calculated the Cronbach’s alpha of the data at the second time point; it also showed sufficient or good internal consistency (DA: α = 0.78; MA: α = 0.80; NC: α = 0.89). The factor structure of the MAAS-J was also consistent with the original version, except for one item that was below 0.40 (the factor loading for item 30 was 0.377).
Table 3 shows the descriptive statistics of each factor in the MAAS-J. Table 3 also includes the means and standard deviations for the Italian sample from the original MAAS. The results showed that the Japanese sample scored higher than the original Italian sample on the two factors DA (t = 10.42, p < 0.01, d = 0.73) and MA (t = 2.25, p = 0.02, d = 0.15), and the Italian sample scored higher than the Japanese sample on NC (t = −11.20, p < 0.01, d = 0.80).
3.3. Examination of Construct Validity
To verify the construct validity of the MAAS-J, we conducted a correlation analysis between the MAAS-J and each scale. The results were generally consistent with our expectations (Table 4). First, the correlations between DA and the scales that were expected to be related to DA were as follows: neuroticism (r = 0.46, p < 0.01), extraversion (r = −0.20, p < 0.01), openness (r = −0.14, p = 0.01), decisiveness (r = −0.40, p < 0.01), PFP (r = 0.27, p < 0.01), PA (r = −0.17, p < 0.01), NA (r = 0.29, p < 0.01), STAI (r = 0.45, p < 0.01), decisiveness (r = −0.40, p < 0.01), and PFO (r = 0.27, p < 0.01), FFMQ (r = −0.37, p < 0.01), observing (r = 0.13, p = 0.01), describing (r = −0.20, p < 0.01), AWA (r = −0.32, p < 0.01), NJ (r = −0.39, p < 0.01), NR (r = −0.28, p < 0.01), SWLS (r = −0.19, p < 0.01), and MWBS (r = −0.16, p < 0.01). In contrast, there was no significant correlation between DA and PFO (r = −0.02, p = 0.70).
![]()
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for MAAS-J subscales and total scores.
Note. The figure “r” shows correlation between day-1 mean and day-2 mean (i.e., re-test reliability).
![]()
Table 4. Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) of the scales used for construct validity, and the correlations with MAAS-J.
Note. Bold indicates a significant difference (p < 0.05). For sex, 1 is male and 2 is female.
The correlations between MA and the predicted scales were significant for AWA (r = −0.11, p = 0.04), NJ (r = −0.14, p < 0.01), and PFO (r = 0.09, p = 0.06), while the correlations between MA and openness (r = 0.04, p = 0.41), decisiveness (r = 0.03, p = 0.54), PFP (r = −0.05, p = 0.35), FFMQ (r = −0.06, p = 0.21), observing (r = 0.03, p = 0.56), describing (r = 0.04, p = 0.45), and NR (r = −0.01, p = 0.91) were not significant. There was also a significant positive correlation between MA and PA (r = 0.14, p < 0.01), and a significant negative correlation between MA and SWLS (r = −0.09, p = 0.09), neither of which were expected.
Finally, the correlations between NC and the predicted scales were as follows: neuroticism (r = −0.11, p = 0.03), extraversion (r = 0.15, p < 0.01), openness (r = 0.44, p < 0.01), decisiveness (r = 0.18, p < 0.01), PFP (r = −0.39, p < 0.01), PA (r = 0.19, p < 0.01), FFMQ (r = 0.21, p < 0.01), observing (r = 0.29, p < 0.01), describing (r = 0.26, p < 0.01), NJ (r = −0.10, p = 0.04), NR (r = 0.19, p < 0.01), SWLS (r = 0.19, p < 0.01), and MWBS (r = 0.15). However, there was no significant correlation between NC and PFO (r = 0.00, p = 0.93), NA (r = 0.02, p = 0.65), STAI (r = −0.04, p = 0.40), and AWA (r = −0.05, p = 0.29).
4. Discussion
4.1. Factor Structure and Reliability
The results of our factor analysis and model comparison showed that the MAAS-J’s structure and items were comparable to the original MAAS. Specifically, the bi-factor model, which assumes three factors (DA, MA, NC) and general factors, showed the best model fit (X2 = 307.91, GFI = 0.931, AGFI = 0.901, NFI = 0.863, CFI = 0.928, RMSEA = 0.048, AIC = 25,700.71). Since the models are similar, we can conclude that the MAAS-J can effectively classify Japanese people’s attitudes toward ambiguity as per the three main factors in the original model and the MAAS-J can be used to examine relationship between these factors and other scales. The MAAS-J also showed good internal reliability (DA: α = 0.73; MA: α = 0.75; NC: α = 0.84) and re-test reliability (DA:r = 0.69; MA: r = 0.73; NC: r = 0.83).
4.2. Construct Validity
Since we found the expected correlations when comparing the MAAS-J with related scales, we consider the MAAS-J to have sufficient construct validity. First, we found that neuroticism in the BFS was moderately correlated with DA and weakly correlated with NC, and that trait anxiety and DA were moderately correlated. The two scales showed a moderate correlation of r > 0.40. These findings are consistent with previous studies on intolerance of uncertainty, which showed that intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety are similar but different (Bardi et al., 2009). Extroversion and openness in the BFS were negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with NC, while MA was not well characterized in BFS, similar to the findings of previous studies (Lauriola et al., 2016).
We also found that NA was positively correlated with DA and that PA was negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with MA and NC. This finding is corroborated by previous studies (Bardi et al., 2009; Iannello et al., 2020) in which intolerance of ambiguity was positively correlated with NA and negatively correlated with PA. The fact that MA was weakly but unexpectedly associated with PA suggests that people whose attitudes favor black-and-white, conclusive, and unambiguous judgements may be more adaptive and associated with PA in Japanese culture, which is driven by conformity to norms.
We also found that well-being was negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with NC, albeit to a weak extent. These results are consistent with the concepts that DA indicates a negative attitude toward ambiguity and NC indicates a positive attitude. Given that the ease with which people hold positive emotions is linked to their well-being (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002), it is reasonable that well-being is negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with NC. Furthermore, our multiple regression analysis with SWLS as the dependent variable and DA and PA as the independent variables showed that the effect of DA on well-being was still significant (b = −0.23, β = −0.13, SE = 0.09, t = −2.72, p < 0.01). Although weakly correlated, the MWBS also showed a negative correlation with DA and a positive correlation with NC. Our finding that people’s attitude toward ambiguity leads to their well-being in relation to the MWBS—which is considered to be a more appropriate measure of Japanese people’s well-being (Kan et al., 2009)—is novel.
We also found that mindfulness as measured by the FFMQ was negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with NC. These results are consistent with previous studies that have shown a relationship between tolerance of ambiguity and mindfulness (Fulton, 2016; Le et al., 2012; Robinson, 2019). Although the five subscales of the FFMQ were correlated generally in the same direction as the total score, it is worth noting that observing was positively correlated with DA and NJ was negatively correlated with both MA and NC. The FFMQ’s bi-factor model of FFMQ does not fit the observing factor, suggesting its specificity (Baer et al., 2006, 2008). Since we first “observe” the subject and then judge the ambiguity as positive or negative, the “observing” factor was positively correlated with both DA and NC. We also found that NJ was negatively correlated with MA, which was not correlated with the FFMQ total score (as hypothesized). This suggests that MA attitudes and mindfulness are opposites. NJ was also negatively (but weakly) correlated with NC, proving that NC represents the epistemic aspect of people’s attitudes toward ambiguity (Lauriola et al., 2016).
Regarding NFC, we found that decisiveness was negatively correlated with DA and positively correlated with NC and that PFP was positively correlated with DA and negatively correlated with NC. The decisiveness subscale included items such as, “I usually make important decisions quickly and confidently,” which may indicate a tendency to avoid or eliminate ambiguity. Therefore, we find it reasonable that decisiveness was associated with DA and NC but not with MA. Similarly, because PFP reflects a preference for predictability, we feel that its association with DA and NC is reasonable as well. Although people’s need for cognitive closure and their intolerance of ambiguity have been thought of as similar concepts (Iannello et al., 2017; Kruglanski & Webster, 1996), the results here confirm the discriminant validity of these concepts as being distinct.
However, not many scales showed a significant correlation with MA, and the construct validity of MA needs to be investigated further. The MA indicates a dichotomous attitude toward ambiguity, so we expected that it would be related to people’s need for cognitive closure. However, in this study, a significant (but weak) correlation was found only between MA and PFO. In the present study, MA was positively correlated with PA (r = 0.14, p < 0.01) and PFP was negatively correlated with PA (r = −0.22, p < 0.01). In other words, the findings suggest that MA is a positive attitude and PFP in NFC is a negative attitude. Therefore, it is possible that MA and NFC were not as highly correlated as expected. Other scholars have suggested that MA is related to prejudice and political attitudes (Forsberg et al., 2019; Lauriola et al., 2016; Moss & O’Connor, 2020), therefore, we expect it to be explored in the future and that its validity will be tested with other scales.
5. Conclusion
This study successfully developed and validated a Japanese version of the MAAS. However, it has a few limitations. Specifically, our work revealed that cultural differences had an impact on our study participants’ responses. Our Japanese study participants had higher DA and MA scores, and Italian study participants from the original MAAS had higher NC scores. The results support the claim that Japan is a so-called tight culture (Mrazek et al., 2013) with a low tolerance for ambiguity compared to Western countries (Gelfand et al., 2011). To date, only a limited number of cross-cultural comparative studies have addressed people’s attitudes toward ambiguity. For example, Spector et al. (2001) examined uncertainty avoidance across 23 countries, although the reliability of the scale used in that study is questionable. In that study, Japan had one of the highest ambiguity avoidance scores. Another study that examined intolerance of ambiguity between Italians and British people (Bottesi et al., 2016) showed that Italians were more tolerant of ambiguity. Thus, cultural comparisons of attitudes toward ambiguity have been made only piecemeal and need to be examined more comprehensively in the future.
This study’s MAAS-J was both structured in a similar way to the original version and had good internal consistency (α = 0.73 - 0.85), re-test reliability (r = 0.69 - 0.83), and construct validity. It was able to newly show correlations between people’s attitudes toward ambiguity and several similar scales (e.g., subscales in the FFMQ and MWBS). Clear differences between Japanese and Italian samples were shown for the first time. We hope that the Japanese version of this scale will assist future cross-cultural comparative research on people’s attitudes toward ambiguity and will help to elucidate the psychological mechanisms behind these attitudes. While the MAAS has been established internationally as an appropriate measure of people’s attitudes toward ambiguity, limited research has been done on the relationship between these attitudes and people’s neural substrates. Considering that functional brain imaging studies of perceived ambiguity have shown that people’s attitudes have emotional, cognitive, and epistemic aspects—and considering that these aspects correlate to the DA MA, and NC subscales (Nomura et al., 2003)—we expect the MAAS-J to contribute to future, comprehensive examinations of this kind. Future researchers could extend our research by confirming the validity of the MAAS-J with behavioral and neural indices currently practically being used in medical and educational settings as well as assessing whether and how interventions work in terms of attitudes toward ambiguity (case method, Banning, 2003; art appreciation, Bentwich & Gilbey, 2017).
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank Marco Lauriola for his help with back translation. This research was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research— KAKENHI—(JSPS KAKENHI Grant Nos. 19H01773).
Supplementary
Developing and Validating a Japanese Version of the Multidimensional Attitude toward Ambiguity Scale (MAAS) Supplementary Tables.
![]()
Table S1. Fit indices of the confirmatory factor analysis for the 30-item and 20-item of MAAS-J.
Note. Chisq = chi square; GFI = goodness of fit index; AGFI = adjusted GFI; NFI = normed fit index; CFI = com-parative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike’s information criterion.
![]()
Table S2. Result of the confirmatory factor analysis for the 30-item and 20-item of MAAS-J.
日本語版Multidimensional Attitude Toward Ambiguity Scale (21 item version)
私たちは,生活の中で特定の状況に直面したときに,人々がどのように反応するかに興味を持っています.このアンケートでは,あなたがこれらの状況を経験したときに,一般的にどのようなことをしているのか,どのように感じているのかを尋ねています.各項目については,他の項目とは独立して回答するようにしてください.回答は,できるだけ自分に当てはまるように考えて選んでください.正解も不正解もなく,「ほとんどの人」がどう感じるかではなく,あなたにとって正確な回答を求めています.各項目の 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 の数字に〇をつけて,それぞれについてあなたの感じていることを表現してください.
1 = 全く同意しない
2 = 同意しない
3 = どちらかといえば同意しない
4 = どちらともいえない
5 = どちらかといえば同意する
6 = 同意する
7 = 強く同意する
因子: 曖昧さへの不快感 (DA; Discomfort With Ambiguity): 3, 5, 8, 10, 12, 16, 20; 道徳的絶対主義/分裂 (MA; Moral Absolutism/Splitting): 1, 4, 6, 9, 14, 18, 21; 複雑性・新規性希求 (NC; Need for Complexity and Novelty): 2, 7, 11, 13, 15, 17, 19.