Physical and Motor Fitness, Sport Skills and Executive Function in Adolescents: A Moderated Prediction Model

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that fitness and sport expertise jointly benefit cognition and that expertise in cognitively demanding strategic sports enhances both domain-specific cognition and domain-general cognitive function, the executive. However, research focusing on whether physical and motor fitness and sport skill are independent determinants of executive function efficiency, or interact with each other is still lacking. The present study investigated this issue in adolescents. Four hundred and eleven boys and girls aged 12 to 15 years were recruited from Italian schools. They were tested for 1) physical fitness (cardiovascular fitness and muscle power); 2) motor control and perceptual-motor adaptation ability (kinaesthetic discrimination and response orientation ability); 3) core executive functions (inhibition and working memory updating); 4) game skills in team sport (decision making and support). While working memory updating was predicted only by physical fitness, inhibition was predicted by game skill, physical fitness and response orientation ability, and by the interaction of these latter ones. Fitness level significantly moderated the prediction accrued by response orientation ability, with inhibition predicted only in the presence of higher physical fitness. The present findings support the view that there are other pathways through which sport practice influences executive function beside the well-known physical fitness/executive function relationship. Alternatives include those linking executive function to the ability to perform coordinated movements in response to environmental cues and to the ability to perform cognitively challenging, strategic actions as needed in sport game situations. Also, the findings highlight that different executive functions are differently linked to physical fitness, motor fitness and sport proficiency.

Share and Cite:

Marchetti, R. , Forte, R. , Borzacchini, M. , Vazou, S. , Tomporowski, P. and Pesce, C. (2015) Physical and Motor Fitness, Sport Skills and Executive Function in Adolescents: A Moderated Prediction Model. Psychology, 6, 1915-1929. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.614189.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
[2] Ainsworth B. E., Haskell, W. L., Whitt, M. C., Irwin, M. L., Swartz, A. M., Strath, S. J. et al. (2000). Compendium of Physical Activities: An Update of Activity Codes and MET Intensities. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 32, 498-504.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005768-200009001-00009
[3] Berchicci, M., Pontifex, M. B., Drollette, E. S., Pesce, C., Hillman, C. H., & Di Russo, F. (2015). From Cognitive Motor Preparation to Visual Processing: The Benefits of Childhood Fitness to Brain Health. Neuroscience, 298, 211-219.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.04.028
[4] Best, J. (2010). Effects of Physical Activity on Children’s Executive Function: Contributions of Experimental Research on Aerobic Exercise. Developmental Review, 30, 331-351.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001
[5] Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. J. (2006). Development of the Adolescent Brain: Implications for Executive Function and Social Cognition. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296-312.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x
[6] Booth, J. N., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, J. M., Ness, A. R., Joinson, C., Leary, S. D., & Reilly, J. J. (2013). Associations between Executive Attention and Objectively Measured Physical Activity in Adolescence: Findings from ALSPAC, a UK Cohort. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 6, 212-219.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2013.09.002
[7] Caprara, G. V. (2001). La valutazione dell’autoefficacia: Costrutti e strumenti. [Self-Efficacy Evaluation: Constructs and Instruments]. Trento: Erikson.
[8] Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Kim, J. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M. et al. (2010). A Neuroimaging Investigation of the Association between Aerobic Fitness, Hippocampal Volume and Memory Performance in Preadolescent Children. Brain Research, 1358, 172-183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.049
[9] Chaddock, L., Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., & Cohen, N. J. (2011). Aerobic Fitness and Executive Control of Relational Memory in Preadolescent Children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 43, 344-349.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181e9af48
[10] Chaddock, L., Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., & Kramer, A. F. (2011). A Review of the Relation of Aerobic Fitness and Physical Activity to Brain Structure and Function in Children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 1-11.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617711000567
[11] Chan, J. S. Y., Wong, A. C. N., Liu, Y., Yu, J., & Yan, J. (2011). Fencing Expertise and Physical Fitness Enhance Action Inhibition. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 509-514.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.04.006
[12] Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding Adolescence as a Period of Social-Affective Engagement and Goal Flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 636-650.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313
[13] Council of the European Union (2013). Council Recommendation on Promoting Health-Enhancing Physical Activity across Sectors. Brussels.
http://ec.europa.eu/sport/library/news-documents/hepa_en.pdf
[14] Diamond, A. (2000). Close Interrelation of Motor Development and Cognitive Development and of the Cerebellum and Prefrontal Cortex. Child Development, 71, 44-56.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00117
[15] Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
[16] Diamond, A. (2015). Effects of Physical Exercise on Executive Functions: Going beyond Simply Moving to Moving with Thought. Annals of Sports Medicine and Research, 2, 1011.
[17] Dumith, S. C., Gigante, D. P., Domingues, M. R., & Kohl, H. V. (2012). Physical Activity Change during Adolescence: A Systematic Review and a Pooled Analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 40, 685-698.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyq272
[18] Esteban-Cornejo, I., Tejero-Gonzalez, C. M., Sallis, J. F., & Veiga, O. L. (2015). Physical Activity and Cognition in Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18, 534-539.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.007
[19] Gallotta, M. C., Marchetti, R., Baldari, C., Guidetti, L., & Pesce, C. (2009). Linking Coordinative and Fitness Training in Physical Education Settings. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, 19, 412-418.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2008.00796.x
[20] Garber, C. E., Blissmer, B., Deschenes, M. R., Franklin, B. A., Lamonte, M. J., Lee, I. M. et al., American College of Sports Medicine (2011). Quantity and Quality of Exercise for Developing and Maintaining Cardiorespiratory, Musculoskeletal, and Neuromotor Fitness in Apparently Healthy Adults: Guidance for Prescribing Exercise. Position Stand. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 43, 1334-1359.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e318213fefb
[21] Gomez-Pinilla, F., & Hillman, C. (2013). The Influence of Exercise on Cognitive Abilities. Comprehensive Physiology, 1, 403-428.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c110063
[22] Hagströmer, M., Bergman, P., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Ortega, F. B., Ruiz, J. R., Manios, Y. et al., The HELENA Study Group (2008). Concurrent Validity of a Modified Version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ-A) in European Adolescents: The HELENA Study. International Journal of Obesity, 32, 342-348.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2008.182
[23] Hall, P. A., Elias, L. J., & Crossley, M. (2006). Neurocognitive Influences on Health Behavior in a Community Sample. Health Psychology, 25, 778-782.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.778
[24] Hallal, P. C., Victora, C. G., Azevedo, M. R., & Wells, J. C. (2006). Adolescent Physical Activity and Health: A Systematic Review. Sports Medicine, 36, 1019-1030.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200636120-00003
[25] Helmerhorst, H. J., Brage, S., Warren, J., Besson, H., & Ekelund, U. (2012). A Systematic Review of Reliability and Objective Criterion-Related Validity of Physical Activity Questionnaires. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9, 103.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-9-103
[26] Herting, M. M., & Nagel, B. J. (2012). Aerobic Fitness Relates to Learning on a Virtual Morris Water Task and Hippocampal Volume in Adolescents. Behavioural Brain Research, 233, 517-525.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.012
[27] Herting, M. M., & Nagel, B. J. (2013). Differences in Brain Activity during a Verbal Associative Memory Encoding Task in High- and Low-Fit Adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 595-612.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00344
[28] Herting, M. M., Colby, J. B., Sowell, E. R., & Nagel, B. J. (2014). White Matter Connectivity and Aerobic Fitness in Male Adolescents. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 65-75.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.003
[29] Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be Smart, Exercise Your Heart: Exercise Effects on Brain and Cognition. Nature Review Neuroscience, 8, 58-65.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2298
[30] Hirtz, P., Arndt, H. J., Holtz, D., Jung, R., Ludwig, G., Schielke, E. et al. (1985). Koordinative Fähigkeiten im Schulsport (Coordinative Abilities in Physical Education). Berlin: Volk und Wissen Verlag.
[31] Howie, E. K., & Pate, R. R. (2012). Physical Activity and Academic Achievement in Children: A Historical Perspective. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 1, 160-169.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2012.09.003
[32] Jacobson, J., & Matthaeus, L. (2014). Athletics and Executive Functioning: How Athletic Participation and Sport Type Correlate with Cognitive Performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15, 521-527.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.005
[33] Khan, N. A., & Hillman, C. H. (2014). The Relation of Childhood Physical Activity and Aerobic Fitness to Brain Function and Cognition: A Review. Pediatric Exercise Science, 26, 138-146.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/pes.2013-0125
[34] Léger, L. A., & Lambert, J. (1982). A Maximal Multistage 20m Shuttle Run Test to Predict VO2max. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 49, 1-12.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00428958
[35] McMorris, T., Tomporowski, P. D., & Audiffren, M. (Eds.) (2009). Exercise and Cognitive Function. West Sussex: Wiley and Sons.
[36] McMorris, T. (In Press). Exercise-Cognition Interaction: A Neuroscience Perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
[37] Mechling, H. H. (1999). Coordinative Abilities. In Y. V. Auweele, F. Bakker, S. Biddle, M. Durand, & R. Selier (Eds.), Psychology for Physical Educators (pp. 159-186). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
[38] Mitchell, S. A., Oslin, J. L., & Griffin, L. L. (2013). Teaching Sport Concepts and Skills: A Tactical Games Approach for Ages 7 to 18 (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
[39] Moran, A. (2009). Cognitive Psychology in Sport: Progress and Prospect. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 420-426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.02.010
[40] Moreau, D. (2015). Unreflective Actions? Complex Motor Skill Acquisition to Enhance Spatial Cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14, 349-359.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9376-9
[41] Moreau, D., & Conway, A. R. A. (2013). Cognitive Enhancement: A Comparative Review of Computerized and Athletic Training Programs. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 6, 155-183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2012.758763
[42] Niederer, I., Kriemler, S., Gu, J., Hartmann, T., Schindler, C., Barra, J., & Puder, J. J. (2011). Relationship of Aerobic Fitness and Motor Skills with Memory and Attention in Preschoolers (Ballabeina): A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Study. BMC Pediatrics, 11, 34.
[43] Ottevaere, C., Huybrechts, I., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Sjöström, M., Ruiz, J. R., Ortega, F. B. et al. (2011). Comparison of the IPAQ-A and Actigraph in Relation to VO2max among European Adolescents: The HELENA Study. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 14, 317-324.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2011.02.008
[44] Pangelinan, M. M., Zhang, G., VanMeter, J. W., Clark, J. E., Hatfield, B. D., & Haufler, A. J. (2011). Beyond Age and Gender: Relationships between Cortical and Subcortical Brain Volume and Cognitive-Motor Abilities in School-Age Children. Neuroimage, 54, 3093-3100.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.021
[45] Pastorelli, C., Caprara G. V., & Bandura, A. (1998). La misura dell’autoefficacia percepita in età scolare (Assessing Perceived Self-Efficacy in Schoolers). Età Evolutiva, 61, 28-40.
[46] Pesce, C. (2012). Shifting the Focus from Quantitative to Qualitative Exercise Characteristics in Exercise and Cognition Research. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 34, 766-786.
[47] Pesce, C., & Ben-Soussan, T. D. (In Press). “Cogito ergo sum” or “ambulo ergo sum”? New Perspectives in Developmental Exercise and Cognition Research. In T. McMorris (Ed.), Exercise-Cognition Interaction: A Neuroscience Perspective. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
[48] Pesce, C., Marchetti, R., Forte, R., Crova, C., Scatigna, M., Goudas, M., & Danish, S. (In Press). Youth Life Skills Training: Exploring Outcomes and Mediating Mechanisms of a Group-Randomized Trial in Physical Education. Sport, Exercise and Performance Psychology, In Press.
[49] Pindus, D. M., Davis, R. D., Hillman, C. H., Bandelow, S., Hogervorst, E., Biddle, S. J., & Sherar, L. B. (2015). The Relationship of Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity to Cognitive Processing in Adolescents: Findings from the ALSPAC Birth Cohort. Psychological Research, 79, 715-728.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0612-2
[50] Pharo, H., Sim, C., Graham, M., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2011). Risky Business: Executive Function, Personality, and Reckless Behavior during Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood. Behavioural Neuroscience, 125, 970-978.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025768
[51] Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J. et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory Fitness and the Flexible Modulation of Cognitive Control in Preadolescent Children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1332-1345.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21528
[52] Raine, L. B., Lee, H. K., Saliba, B. J., Chaddock-Heyman, L., Hillman, C. H. et al. (2013). The Influence of Childhood Aerobic Fitness on Learning and Memory. PloS ONE, 8, e72666.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072666
[53] Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Kane, R., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012a). An Examination of the Relationship between Motor Coordination and Executive Functions in Adolescents. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54, 1025-1031.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04403.x
[54] Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Kane, R., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012b). Motor Coordination, Working Memory, and Academic Achievement in a Normative Adolescent Sample: Testing a Mediation Model. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, 766-780.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs061
[55] Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., & Jäger, K. (2014). The Relation between Cognitive and Motor Performance and Their Relevance for Children’s Transition to School: A Latent Variable Approach. Human Movement Science, 33, 284-297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.011
[56] Ruiz, J. R., Ortega, F. B., Castillo, R., Martin-Matillas, M., Kwak, L., Vicente-Rodriguez, G. et al. (2010). Physical Activity, Fitness, Weight Status, and Cognitive Performance in Adolescents. The Journal of Pediatrics, 157, 917-922.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.06.026
[57] Singh, A., Uijtdewilligen, L., Twisk, J. W. R., van Mechelen, W., & Chinapaw, M. J. M. (2012). Physical Activity and Performance at School: A Systematic Review of the Literature including a Methodological Quality Assessment. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 166, 49-55.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.716
[58] Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and Affective Development in Adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 69-74.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005
[59] Stroth, S., Kubesch, S., Dieterle, K., Ruchsowd, M., Heim, R., & Kiefer, M. (2009). Physical Fitness, but Not Acute Exercise Modulates Event-Related Potential Indices for Executive Control in Healthy Adolescents. Brain Research, 1269, 114-124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.073
[60] The Cooper Institute (2010). Fitnessgram/Activitygram Test Administration Manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
[61] The IPAQ Group (2015). Guidelines for Data Processing and Analysis of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire. http://www.ipaq.ki.se
[62] Tomporowski, P. D., Lambourne, K., & Okumura, M. S. (2011). Physical Activity Interventions and Children’s Mental Function: An Introduction and Overview. Preventive Medicine, 52, 3-9.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.028
[63] Tomporowski, P. D., McCullick, B., Pendleton, D. M., & Pesce, C. (2015). Exercise and Children’s Cognition: The Role of Exercise Characteristics and a Place for Metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 4, 47-55.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003
[64] Towse, J. N. (1998). On Random Generation and the Central Executive of Working Memory. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 77-101.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02674.x
[65] Towse, J. N., & Cheshire, A. (2007). Random Number Generation and Working Memory. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 374-394.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440600764570
[66] Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing Human Number Generation Behavior: A Review of Methods Used and a Computer Program for Describing Performance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computer, 30, 583-591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209475
[67] Verburgh, L., Königs, M., Scherder, E. J., & Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Physical Exercise and Executive Functions in Preadolescent Children, Adolescents and Young Adults: A Meta-Analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48, 973-979.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441
[68] Verburgh, L., Scherder, E. J. A., van Lange, P. A., & Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Executive Functioning in Highly Talented Soccer Players. PLoS ONE, 9, e91254.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091254
[69] Vestberg, T., Gustafson, R., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., & Petrovic, P. (2012). Executive Functions Predict the Success of Top-Soccer Players. PLoS ONE, 7, e34731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034731
[70] Voss, M. W., Kramer, A. F., Basak, C., Prakash, R. S., & Roberts, B. (2010). Are Expert Athletes “Expert” in the Cognitive Laboratory? A Meta-Analytic Study of Cognition and Sport Expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 812-826.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1588
[71] Wang, C. H., Chang, C. C., Liang, Y. M., Shih, C. M., Chiu, W. S., Tseng, P. et al. (2013) Open vs. Closed Skill Sports and the Modulation of Inhibitory Control. PloS ONE, 8, e55773.
[72] Williams, A. M., & Ericcson, K. A. (2005). Perceptual-Cognitive Expertise in Sport: Some Considerations When Applying the Expert Performance Approach. Human Movement Science, 24, 283-307.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.002
[73] Young, J., Angevaren, M., Rusted, J., & Tabet, N. (2015). Aerobic Exercise to Improve Cognitive Function in Older People without Known Cognitive Impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 4, Article ID: CD005381.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/14651858.cd005381.pub4
[74] Zinke, K., Einert, M., Pfenni, L., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Plasticity of Executive Control through Task Switching Training in Adolescents. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-15.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00041

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.