Responsibility Attitude and Stimulus Valence in Relation to Recognition and Confidence in Recognition of Words

Abstract

Perceived responsibility may have significant cognitive consequences for both clinical and non-clinical groups. Under conditions of increased responsibility, individuals have been found to show a positive memory bias, but also a decline in metamemory for salient stimuli. Stimulus salience enhances retrieval by means of directive attention, but may actually impede metamemory, especially among anxious populations. It has been suggested that for OCD patients and nonclinical participants with OCD symptoms, progressive exposure to emotionally salient stimuli worsens confidence in memory, while having no effect in memory accuracy. Perceived responsibility is associated with a positive memory bias for negative stimuli with reduced memory confidence. The current study investigated the possible association in a healthy population among responsibility, measured by the Responsibility Attitude Scale (RAS), recognition and confidence in recognition of words that varied in valence. 85 healthy participants were administered the RAS prior to taking part in a word recognition task. Results indicated that responsibility attitude did not predict memory accuracy or memory confidence for negatively, positively or neutrally valenced words. Furthermore, word valence had no effect on memory confidence but did have a significant effect on memory accuracy. Implications for future research point towards the utilisation of responsibility-relevant stimuli.

Share and Cite:

Manoussaki, K. (2015) Responsibility Attitude and Stimulus Valence in Relation to Recognition and Confidence in Recognition of Words. Psychology, 6, 1159-1167. doi: 10.4236/psych.2015.69114.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Ashbaugh, A. R., Gelfand, L. A., & Radomsky, A. S. (2006). Interpersonal Aspects of Responsibility and Obsessive Compulsive Symptoms. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34, 151-163.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465805002699
[2] Boschen, R. M., & Vuksanovic, D. (2007). Deteriorating Memory Confidence, Responsibility Perceptions and Repeated Checking: Comparisons in OCD and Control Samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2098-2109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.009
[3] Bradley, M. M., & Land, P. J. (2010). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. Technical Report C-2. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
[4] Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanism of Emotional Arousal and Lasting Declarative Memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 294-299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01214-9
[5] Ceschi, G., Van der Linden, M., Dunker, D., Perroud, A., & Brédart, S. (2003). Further Exploration Memory Bias in Compulsive Washers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 737-747.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00040-8
[6] Cougle, J. R., Lee, H.-J., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2007). Are Responsibility Beliefs Inflated in Non-Checking OCD Patients? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 153-159.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.012
[7] Cougle, J. R., Salkovskis, P. M., & Wahl, K. (2007) Perception of Memory Ability and Confidence in Recollections in Obsessive-Compulsive Checking. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 118-130.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.015
[8] Dek, E. C. P., Van den Hout, M. A., Giele, C. L., & Engelhard, I. M. (2014). Automatization and Familiarity in Repeated Checking. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3, 303-310.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.07.001
[9] Dewhurst, S. A., & Perry, L. A. (2000). Emotionality, Distinctiveness and Recollective Experience. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 541-551.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/095414400750050222
[10] Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. P. (2001). Source Memory Enhancement for Emotional Words. Emotion, 1, 5-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.5
[11] Ecker, W., & Engelkamp, J. (1995). Memory for Actions in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 349-371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1352465800016477
[12] Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1993). What Do Patients Do with Their Obsessive Thoughts? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 335-348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00094-0
[13] Ghetti, S. (2003). Memory for Nonoccurrences: The Role of Metacognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 722-739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00005-6
[14] Hermans, D., Martens, K., De Cort, K., Pieters, G., & Eelen, P. (2003). Reality Monitoring and Metacognitive Beliefs Related to Cognitive Confidence in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 383-401.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00015-3
[15] Hertel, P. T., & Parks, C. (2002). Emotional Episodes Facilitate Word Recall. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 685-694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000491
[16] Van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2003). Repeated Checking Causes Memory Distrust. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 301-316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00012-8
[17] Karadag, F., Oguzhanoglu, N., Ozdel, O., Atesci, F. C., & Amuk, T. (2005). Memory Function in Patients with Obsessive Compulsive Disorder and the Problem of Confidence in Their Memories: A Clinical Study. Croatian Medical Journal, 46, 282-287.
[18] Kensinger, E. A. (2007). Negative Emotion Enhances Memory Accuracy. Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 213-218.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x
[19] Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory Enhancement for Emotional Words: Are Emotional Words More Vividly Remembered than Neutral Words? Memory and Cognition, 31, 1169-1180.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195800
[20] Lind, C., & Boschen, M. J. (2009). Intolerance of Anxiety Mediates the Effect of Responsibility Beliefs on Compulsive Checking. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1047-1052.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.005
[21] Mancini, F., & Gangemi, A. (2004). Fear of Guilt from Behaving Irresponsibly in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 109-120.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.003
[22] Mancini, F., D’Olimpio, F., & D’Ercole, S. (2001). Responsibility Attitude, Obsession and Compulsion: Further Support in a Non-Clinical Sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 8, 274-281.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.292
[23] Melcher, D., & Piazza, M. (2011). The Role of Attentional Priority and Saliency in Determining Capacity Limits in Enumeration and Visual Working Memory. PLoS ONE, 6, e29296.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029296
[24] Moritz, S., Wahl, K., Zurowski, B., Jelinek, L., Hand, I., & Fricke, S. (2007). Enhanced Perceived Responsibility Decreases Metamemory but Not Memory Accuracy in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2044-2052. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.03.003
[25] Muller, J., & Roberts, J. E. (2005). Memory and Attention in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: A Review. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 1-28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.12.001
[26] Nothdurft, H. C. (2002). Attention Shifts to Salient Targets. Vision Research, 42, 1287-1306.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00016-0
[27] Olley, A., Malhi, G., & Sachdev, P. (2007). Memory and Executive Functioning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Selective Review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 104, 15-23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.02.023
[28] Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are Affective Events Richly “Remembered” or Simply Familiar? The Experience and Process of Recognizing Feelings Past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 242-261.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.242
[29] Rachman, S. (2002). A Cognitive Theory of Compulsive Checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 625-639. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00028-6
[30] Radomsky, A. S., Gilchrist, P. T., & Dussault, D. (2006). Repeated Checking Really Does Cause Memory Distrust. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 44, 305-316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.02.005
[31] Radomsky, A. S., & Rachman, S. (1999). Memory Bias in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 605-618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00151-X
[32] Radomsky, A. S., Rachman, S., & Hammond, D. (2001). Memory Bias, Confidence and Responsibility in Compulsive Checking. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 813-822.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00079-6
[33] Rheaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Dugas, M. J., Letarte, H., & Ladouceur, R. (1995). Perfectionism, Responsibility and Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 785-794.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(95)00017-R
[34] Rubenstein, C. S., Peynircioglu, Z. F., Chambless, D. L., & Pigott, T. A. (1993). Memory in Sub-Clinical Obsessive-Compulsive Checkers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 759-765.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(93)90006-G
[35] Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-Compulsive Problems: A Cognitive Behavioural Analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571-583. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6
[36] Salkovskis, P. M. (1996). Cognitive-Behavioural Approaches to the Understanding of Obsessional Problems. In R. Rapee (Ed.), Current Controversies in the Anxiety Disorders. New York: Guilford.
[37] Salkovskis, P. M., Rachman, S., Ladouceur, R., & Freeston, M. (1992). The Definition of Responsibility. Paper Presented at the World Congress of Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies, Toronto.
[38] Salkovskis, P. M., Wroe, A. L., Gledhill, A., Morrison, N. Forrester, E., Richards, C. Reynolds, M., & Thorpe, S. (2000). Responsibility Attitudes and Interpretations Are Characteristic of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 347-372.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00071-6
[39] Sher, K., Frost, R. O., Kushner, M., Crews, T. M., & Alexander, J. E. (1989). Memory Deficits in Compulsive Checkers: Replication and Extension in a Clinical Sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 493-502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(84)90053-6
[40] Sher, K., Frost, R. O., & Otto, R. (1983). Cognitive Deficits in Compulsive Checkers: An Exploratory Study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 357-363.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(83)90004-9
[41] Strack, F., & Bless, H. (1994). Memory for Nonoccurences: Metacognitive and Presuppositional Strategies. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 203-217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1010
[42] Tallis, F. (1997). The Neuropsychology of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: A Review and Consideration of Clinical Implications. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 3-20.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01226.x
[43] Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S, Brigidi, B. D., Amir, N., Street, G. P., & Foa, E. B. (2001). Memory and Memory Confidence in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 913-927.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00064-4

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.