[1]
|
Anderson, J. M., & Ewen, C. J. (1987). Principles of Dependency Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[2]
|
Bordag, D., Opitz, A., & Pechmann, T. (2006). Gender Processing in L1 and L2: The Role of Noun Termination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1090-1101.
|
[3]
|
Charette, M. (1991). Conditions on Phonological Government. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[4]
|
Chen, J.-Y., Chen, T.-M., & Dell, G. S. (2002). Wordform Encoding in Mandarin Chinese as Assessed by the Implicit Priming Task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 751-781.
|
[5]
|
Cutler, A. (1976). Phoneme-Monitoring as a Function Of Preceding Intonation Contour. Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 55-60.
|
[6]
|
Cutler, A., & Otake, T. (1994). Mora or Phoneme? Further Evidence for Language-Specific Listening. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 824-844.
|
[7]
|
De Groot, A. M. B., Delmaar, P., & Lupker, S. J. (2000). The Processing of Interlexical Homographs in Translation Recognition and Lexical Decision: Support for Non-Selective Access to Bilingual Memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 53A, 397-428.
|
[8]
|
Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (1999). Recognition of Cognates and Interlingual Homographs: The Neglected Role of Phonology. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 496-518.
|
[9]
|
Dijkstra, T., Roelofs, A. & Fieuws, S. (1995). Orthographic Effects on Phoneme Monitoring. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 264-271.
|
[10]
|
Dijkstra, T., Timmermans, M., & Schriefers, H. (2000). On Being Blinded by Your Other Language: Effects of Task Demands on Interlingual Homograph Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 445-464.
|
[11]
|
Frauenfelder, U. H., & Segui, J. (1989). Phoneme Monitoring and Lexical Processing: Evidence for Associative Context Effects. Memory and Cognition, 17, 134-140. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03197063
|
[12]
|
Harris, J. (1994). English Sound Structure. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
|
[13]
|
Harris, J. (1997). Licensing Inheritance: An Integrated Theory of Neutralisation. Phonology, 14, 315-370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0952675798003479
|
[14]
|
Harris, J. (2004). Release the Captive Coda: The Foot as a Domain of Phonetic Interpretation. In J. K. Local, R. Ogden, & R. A. M. Temple (Eds.), Phonetic Interpretation (pp. 103-129). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[15]
|
Harris, J., & Gussmann, E. (1998). Final Codas: Why the West Was Wrong. In E. Cyran (Ed.), Structure and Interpretation: Studies in Phonology (pp. 139-162). Lublin: Folium.
|
[16]
|
Harris, J., & Gussmann, E. (2002). Word-Final Onsets. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 14, 1-42.
|
[17]
|
Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do Bilinguals Activate Phonological Representations in One or Both of Their Languages When Naming Words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 2-31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2747
|
[18]
|
Jared, D., & Szucs, C. (2002). Phonological Activation in Bilinguals: Evidence from Interlingual Homograph Naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 225-239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728902003024
|
[19]
|
Kaye, J. D. (1990). “Coda” Licensing. Phonology, 7, 301-330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700001214
|
[20]
|
Kaye, J. D., Lowenstamm, J., & Vergnaud, J.-R. (1990). Constituent Structure and Government in Phonology. Phonology, 7, 193-232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700001184
|
[21]
|
Kroll, J. F., & de Groot, A. M. B. (1997). Lexical and Conceptual Memory in the Bilingual: Mapping Form to Meaning in Two Languages. In A. M. B. De Groot, & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Perspectives (pp. 169-199). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.
|
[22]
|
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections between Bilingual Memory Representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1008
|
[23]
|
Kroll, J. F., & Tokowicz, N. (2005). Models of Bilingual Representation and Processing: Looking Back and to the Future. In J. F. Kroll, & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (pp. 531-553). New York: Oxford University Press.
|
[24]
|
Kureta, Y., Fushimi, T., & Tatsumi, I. F. (2006). The Functional Unit of Phonological Encoding: Evidence for Moraic Representation in Native Japanese Speakers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1102-1119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1102
|
[25]
|
Lemhofer, K., Spalek, K., & Schriefers, H. (2008). Cross-Language Effects of Grammatical Gender in Bilingual Word Recognition and Production. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 312-330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.005
|
[26]
|
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A Theory of Lexical Access in Speech Production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776
|
[27]
|
Meyer, A. S. (1990). The Time Course of Phonological Encoding in Language Production: The Encoding of Successive Syllables of a Word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524-545. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90050-A
|
[28]
|
Meyer, A. S. (1991). The Time Course of Phonological Encoding in Language Production: Phonological Encoding inside a Syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90011-8
|
[29]
|
Nasukawa, K. (1998). Prosodic Integrity and Melodic Representation in Nasals. Interdisciplinary Information Sciences, 4, 1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.4036/iis.1998.1
|
[30]
|
Nasukawa, K. (2004). Word-Final Consonants: Arguments against a Coda Analysis. Proceedings of the 58th Conference, Tohoku English Literary Society, 47-53.
|
[31]
|
Nasukawa, K. (2011). Representing Phonology without Precedence Relations. English Linguistics, 28, 278-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.9793/elsj.28.2_278
|
[32]
|
Nasukawa, K. (2014). Features and Recursive Structure. Nordlyd, 41, 1-19.
|
[33]
|
Nasukawa, K., Yasugi, Y., & Koizumi, M. (2013). Syllable Structure and the Head Parameter in Kaqchikel. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), Studies in Kaqchikel Grammar (pp. 81-95). MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
|
[34]
|
Newman, J. E., & Dell, G. S. (1978). The Phonological Nature of Phoneme Monitoring: A Critique of Some Ambiguity Studies. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 359-374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90228-1
|
[35]
|
Ozdemir, R., Roelofs, A., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2007). Perceptual Uniqueness Point Effects in Monitoring Internal Speech. Cognition, 105, 457-465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.006
|
[36]
|
Roelofs, A. (1996). Serial Order in Planning the Production of Successive Morphemes of a Word. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 854-876. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0044
|
[37]
|
Roelofs, A. (2008). Tracing Attention and the Activation Flow in Spoken Word Planning Using Eye Movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 353-368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.353
|
[38]
|
Roelofs, A. (2015). Modeling of Phonological Encoding in Spoken Word Production: From Germanic Languages to Mandarin Chinese and Japanese. Japanese Psychological Research, 57, 22-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12050
|
[39]
|
Salamoura, A., & Williams, J. N. (2007). The Representation of Grammatical Gender in the Bilingual Lexicon: Evidence from Greek and German. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 257-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003069
|
[40]
|
Segui, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Mehler, J. (1981). Phoneme Monitoring, Syllable Monitoring and Lexical Access. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 471-477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1981.tb01776.x
|
[41]
|
Sholl, A., Sankaranarayanan, A., & Kroll, J. F. (1995). Transfer between Picture Naming and Translation: A Test of Asymmetries in Bilingual Memory. Psychological Science, 6, 45-49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00303.x
|
[42]
|
Van der Hulst, H. (1995). Radical CV Phonology: The Categorial Gesture. In J. Durand, & F. Katamba (Eds.), Frontiers of Phonology: Atoms, Structures, Derivations (pp. 80-116). Harlow, Essex: Longman.
|
[43]
|
Van Heuven, J. B., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J. (1998). Orthographic Neighborhood Effects in Bilingual Word Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 458-483. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2584
|
[44]
|
Wheeldon, L. R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995) Monitoring the Time Course of Phonological Encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 311-334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1014
|