[1]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (1995). Learning to Read in Arabic: Reading, Syntactic, Orthographic and Working Memory Skills in Normally Achieving and Poor Arabic Readers. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 16, 351-394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0270271950160401
|
[2]
|
Abu Rabia, S. (2001). The Role of Vowels in Reading Semitic Scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 39-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008147606320
|
[3]
|
Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2002). Reading, Syntactic, Orthographic, and Working Memory Skills of Bilingual Arabic-English Speaking Canadian Children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 661-678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1021221206119
|
[4]
|
Abu Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word Recognition and Basic Cognitive Processes among Reading-Disabled and Normal Readers of Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 423-442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024237415143
|
[5]
|
Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and Spelling Error Analysis of Native Arabic Dyslexic Readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 651-690. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-2657-x
|
[6]
|
Abu-Rabia, S. (2007). The Role of Morphology and Short Vowelization in Reading Arabic among Normal and Dyslexic Readers in Grades 3, 6, 9, and 12. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 89-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10936-006-9035-6
|
[7]
|
Abu-Rabia, S., & Sammour, R. (2013). Spelling Errors’ Analysis of Regular and Dyslexic Bilingual Arabic-English Students. Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 3, 58-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ojml.2013.31007
|
[8]
|
Al Mannai, H. (2006). Cognitive Predictors of Early Arabic Literacy: Informing the Development of Dyslexia Assessment in Bahrain. Ph.D. Thesis, Surrey: Surrey University.
|
[9]
|
Al Mannai, H., & Everatt, J. (2005). Ponological Processing Skills as Preditors of Literacy amongst Arabic Speaking Bahrini Children. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 11, 269-291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.303
|
[10]
|
Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the Predictive Roles of Working Memory and IQ in Academic Attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003
|
[11]
|
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., Text Rev.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
|
[12]
|
Ben-Dror, I., Frost, R., & Bentin, S. (1995). Orthographic Representation and Phonemic Segmentation in Skilled Readers: A Cross-Language Comparison. Psychological Science, 6, 176-181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00328.x
|
[13]
|
Bosse, M.-L., Valdois, S., & Tainturier, M.-J. (2004). Cognitive Deficits Related to Developmental Dyslexia: A French and English Group Study. 6th BDA Conference, Warwich, 27-30 March 2004.
|
[14]
|
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2010). Aralex: A Lexical Database for Modern Standard Arabic. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 481-487. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.481
|
[15]
|
Bowers, P. G., & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The Role of Naming Speed within a Model of Reading Acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 109-126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1013820421199
|
[16]
|
Breznitz, Z. (2008). The Origin of Dyslexia: The Asynchrony Phenomenon. In G. Reid, A. Fawcett, F. Manis, & L. Siegel (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Dyslexia (pp. 11-30). London: SAGE Publications Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9780857020987.n2
|
[17]
|
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s Reading Comprehension Ability: Concurrent Prediction by Working Memory, Verbal Ability, and Component Skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31
|
[18]
|
Caravolas, M., Lervag, A., Mousikou, P., Efrim, C., Litavsky, M., Onochi-Quintanilla, E. et al. (2012). Common Patterns of Prediction of Literacy Development in Different Alphabetic Orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678-686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434536
|
[19]
|
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of Working Memory in Explaining the Performance of Individuals with Specific Reading Comprehension Difficulties: A Meta-Analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246-251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.10.002
|
[20]
|
Cartwright, K. B. (2012). Insights from Cognitive Neuroscience: The Importance of Executive Function for Early Reading Development and Education. Early Education and Development, 23, 24-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.615025
|
[21]
|
Casco, C., Tressoldi, P., & Dellantonio, A. (1998). Visual Selective Attention and Reading Efficiency Are Related in Children. Cortex, 34, 531-546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70512-4
|
[22]
|
Chung, K. K. H., Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S. H. (2011). Cognitive Skills and Literacy Performance of Chinese Adolescents with and without Dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 24, 835-859. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9227-1
|
[23]
|
Cutting, L. E., Materek, A., Cole, C. A. S., Levine, T. M., & Mahone, E. M. (2009). Effects of Fluency, Oral Language, and Executive Function on Reading Comprehension Performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 34-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-009-0022-0
|
[24]
|
De Beni, R., Palladino, P., Pazzaglia, F., & Cornoldi, C. (1998). Increases in Intrusion Errors and Working Memory Deficit of Poor Comprehenders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 305-320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755761
|
[25]
|
Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy Ability and Phonological Processing Skills amongst Dyslexic and Non-Dyslexic Speakers of Arabic. Reading and Writing, 20, 273-294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9031-0
|
[26]
|
Elbeheri, G., Everatt, J., Reid, G., & Al Mannai, H. (2006). Dyslexia Assessment in Arabic. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 6, 143-152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-3802.2006.00072.x
|
[27]
|
Elbeheri, G., Everatt, J., Mahfoudhi, A., Abu Al-Diyar, M., & Taibah, N. (2011). Orthographic Processing and Reading Comprehension among Arabic Speaking Mainstream and LD Children. Dyslexia, 17, 123-142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.430
|
[28]
|
Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual-Spatial Attention in Developmental Dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109-123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70840-2
|
[29]
|
Ferretti, G., Mazzotti, S., & Brizzolara, D. (2008). Visual Scanning and Reading Ability in Normal and Dyslexic Children. Behavioural Neurology, 19, 87-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2008/564561
|
[30]
|
Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 814-819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013
|
[31]
|
Frost, J., Madsbjerg, S., Niedersøe, J., Olofsson, A., & Sørensen, P. M. (2005). Semantic and Phonological Skills in Predicting Reading Development: From 3-16 Years of Age. Dyslexia, 11, 79-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.292
|
[32]
|
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting Reading and Spelling Difficulties in Transparent and Opaque Orthographies: A Comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian Children. Dyslexia, 16, 119-142.
|
[33]
|
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological Awareness and Rapid Automatized Naming Predicting Early Development in Reading and Spelling: Results from a Cross-Linguistic Longitudinal Study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 85-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.005
|
[34]
|
Garson, D. G. (2008). Factor Analysis: Statnotes. North Carolina State University Public Administration Program. http://www.stat.fsu.edu~jfrade/HOMEWORKS/STA5707/STA5707fall07/files/ project/PA%20765_%20Factor%20Analysis.pdf
|
[35]
|
Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Kirkwood, H. J., Elliott, J. G., Holmes, J., & Hilton, K. A. (2008). Attentional and Executive Function Behaviors in Children with Poor Working Memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 214-223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.10.003
|
[36]
|
Georgiou, G., Das, J. P., & Hayward, D. (2008). Comparing the Contribution of Two Tests of Working Memory to Reading in Relation to Phonological Awareness and Rapid Naming Speed. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 302-318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.00373.x
|
[37]
|
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of Word Decoding and Reading Fluency across Languages Varying in Orthographic Consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 566-580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566
|
[38]
|
Georgiou, G. K., Papadopoulos, T. C., Zarouna, E., & Parrila, R. (2012). Are Auditory and Visual Processing Deficits Related to Developmental Dyslexia? Dyslexia, 18, 110-129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.1439
|
[39]
|
Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., Anderson, R., & Tatham, R. (2006). Multivariate Data Analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
|
[40]
|
Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired Visual Processing of Multi-Element Arrays Is Associated with Increased Number of Eye Movements in Dyslexic Reading. Vision Research, 45, 855-863. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.007
|
[41]
|
Hayes-Harb, R. (2006). Native Speakers of Arabic and ESL Texts: Evidence for the Transfer of Written Word Identification Processes. TESOL Quarterly, 40, 321-339. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40264525
|
[42]
|
Huang, H. C., & Wang, T. Y. (2009). Stimulus Effects on Cancellation Task Performance in Children with and without Dyslexia. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 539-545. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.539
|
[43]
|
Iles, J., Walsh, V., & Richardson, A. (2000). Visual Search Performance in Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 6, 163-177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1099-0909(200007/09)6:3<163::AID-DYS150>3.0.CO;2-U
|
[44]
|
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working Memory: Its Role in Dyslexia and Other Specific Learning Difficulties. Dyslexia, 10, 196-214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.278
|
[45]
|
Jiménez, J. E., & Hernández-Valle, I. (2000). Word Identification and Reading Disorders in the Spanish Language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 267-275.
|
[46]
|
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., & Parrila, R. (2010). Naming Speed and Reading: From Prediction to Instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341-362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.3.4
|
[47]
|
Khomsi, A. (1999). LMC-R: Epreuve d'évaluation de la Compétence en Lecture. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
|
[48]
|
Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in Phoneme Segmentation Are Not the Core Problem of Dyslexia: Evidence from German and English Children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243-262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400002058
|
[49]
|
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppanen, P. H. T., Lohvansuu, K. et al. (2013). Predictors of Developmental Dyslexia in European Orthographies with Varying Complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686-694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12029
|
[50]
|
Layes, S., Lalonde, R., & Rebai, M. (in Press). Reading Speed and Phonological Awareness Deficits among Arabic-Speaking Dyslexic Children. Dyslexia. Published online in Wiley Online Library. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.1491
|
[51]
|
Lima, R. F., Azoni, C. A., & Ciasca, S. M. (2010). Attentional Performance and Executive Functions in Children with Learning Difficulties. Psicologia: Reflexao e Crítica, 24, 685-691.
|
[52]
|
Lopez-Escribano, C., & Katzir, T. (2008). Are Phonological Processes Separate from the Processes Underlying Naming Speed in a Shallow Orthography? Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 6, 641-666.
|
[53]
|
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid Naming and the Longitudinal Prediction of Reading Subskills in First and Second Graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129-157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0302_3
|
[54]
|
Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme Awareness and Pathways into Literacy: A Comparison of German and American Children. Reading and Writing, 15, 653-682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781
|
[55]
|
Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working Memory Deficits in Poor Comprehenders Reflect Underlying Language Impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139-158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2498
|
[56]
|
Navarro, J. I., Aguilar, M., Alcalde, C., Ruiz, G., Marchena, E., & Menacho, I. (2011). Inhibitory Processes, Working Memory, Phonological Awareness, Naming Speed, and Early Arithmetic Achievement. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, 580-588. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.6
|
[57]
|
Neuhaus, G. F., & Swank, P. R. (2002). Understanding the Relations between RAN Letter Subtest Components and Word Reading in First-Grade Students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 158-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940203500206
|
[58]
|
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Comparison of Deficits in Cognitive and Motor Skills among Children with Dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 147-164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02648159
|
[59]
|
Oakhill, J., & Kyle, F. (2000). The Relation between Phonological Awareness and Working Memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 152-164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2529
|
[60]
|
Oakhill, J., Yuill, N., & Garnham, A. (2011). The Differential Relations between Verbal, Numerical and Spatial Working Memory Abilities and Children’s Reading Comprehension. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 4, 83-106.
|
[61]
|
Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & McQuarrie, L. (2004). Articulation Rate, Naming Speed, Verbal Short-Term Memory, and Phonological Awareness: Longitudinal Predictors of Early Reading Development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 3-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_2
|
[62]
|
Pazzaglia, F., Cornoldi, C., & Tessoldi, P. E. (1993). Learning to Read: Evidence on the Distinction between Decoding and Comprehension Skills. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 247-258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03174080
|
[63]
|
Protopapas, A., & Altani, A. (2013). RAN Backward: A Test of The Visual Scanning Hypothesis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 453-461. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.769556
|
[64]
|
Protopapas, A., Altani, A., & Georgiou, G. K. (2013). Development of Serial Processing in Reading and Rapid Naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 914-929. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.004
|
[65]
|
Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L., Mencl, E., Austin, A. C., Sibley, D. et al. (2012). The Relationship between Phonological and Auditory Processing and Brain Organization in Beginning Readers. Brain & Language, 125, 173-183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004
|
[66]
|
Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., Wade-Woolley, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a Comprehensive View of the Skills Involved in Word Reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 96-113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004
|
[67]
|
Ryan, A., & Meara, P. (1991). The Case of the Invisible Vowels: Arabic Speakers Reading English Words. Reading in a Foreign Language, 7, 531-540.
|
[68]
|
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of Reading Fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and Orthographic Factors. Reading and Writing, 18, 559-582. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3180-4
|
[69]
|
Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M.-F., Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. M. (2000). Working Memory Resources and Children’s Reading Comprehension. Reading and Writing, 13, 81-103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008088230941
|
[70]
|
Shalev, L., & Tsal, Y. (2003). The Wide Attentional Window: A Major Deficit of Children with Attention Difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 517-527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194030360060301
|
[71]
|
Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to Read and Write in Hebrew. In M. Harris, & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to Read and Write (pp. 89-111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
|
[72]
|
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301-1309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043
|
[73]
|
Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (2003). Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (2nd ed.). Administration and Technical Manual, Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
|
[74]
|
Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Powell, D., Quinlan, P., & Garwood, H. (2010). Visual Processing Deficits in Children with Slow RAN Performance. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 266-292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888431003724070
|
[75]
|
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic Performance Profile of Children with Reading Disabilities: A Regression-Based Test of the Phonological-Core Variable-Difference Model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.24
|
[76]
|
Stein, J., Talcott, J., & Walsh, V. (2000). Controversy about the Evidence for a Visual Magno-Cellular Deficit in Developmental Dyslexics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 209-211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01484-4
|
[77]
|
Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To See but Not to Read: The Magnocellular Theory of Dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 147-152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01005-3
|
[78]
|
Streiner, D., & Norman, G. (1995). Health Measurement Scales: A Practical Guide to Their Development and Use (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
|
[79]
|
Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1995). Individual Differences in Children’s Working Memory and Writing Skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 358-385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0054
|
[80]
|
Sweet, A. P., & Snow, C. E. (Eds.) (2003). Rethinking Reading Comprehension. New York: Guilford.
|
[81]
|
Taha, H. Y. (2013). Investigating Cognitive Processes Underlying Reading in Arabic: Evidence from Typical and Poor Reading Performance. Psychology, 4, 1018-1026. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.412148
|
[82]
|
Taiba, N., Al Behairi, G., Abo Al-dyiar, M., Mahfoudhi, A., Everat, J., & Heinz, C. (2010). Standardized Test of Phonological Processes for Children. Kuwait: Centre of Children Evaluation and Teaching.
|
[83]
|
Taroyan, N. A., Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2007). Behavioural and Neurophysiological Correlates of Dyslexia in the Continuous Performance Task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 845-855. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.11.273
|
[84]
|
Vaessen, A., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Faísca, L., Reis, A. et al. (2010). Cognitive Development of Fluent Word Reading Does Not Qualitatively Differ between Transparent and Opaque Orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 827-842. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019465
|
[85]
|
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific Reading Disability (Dyslexia): What Have We Learned in the Past Four Decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x
|
[86]
|
Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: A Deficit in Visuo-Spatial Attention, Not in Phonological Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 57-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003
|
[87]
|
Visser, T., Boden, C., & Giaschi, D. (2004). Children with Dyslexia: Evidence for Visual Attention Deficits in Perception of Rapid Sequences of Objects. Vision Research, 44, 2521-2535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.010
|
[88]
|
Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). The Double-Deficit Hypothesis: A Comprehensive Analysis of Evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 25-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194060390010401
|
[89]
|
Wiig, E. H., Zureich, P., & Helen Chan, H. N. (2000). A Clinical Rationale for Assessing Rapid Automatized Naming in Children with Language Disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 359-374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221940003300407
|
[90]
|
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The Double-Deficit Hypothesis and Difficulties in Learning to Read a Regular Orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668-680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668
|
[91]
|
Wolf, M. (1986). Rapid Alternating Stimulus Naming in the Developmental Dyslexias. Brain and Language, 27, 360-379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(86)90025-8
|
[92]
|
Wolf, M. (1991). Naming Speed and Reading: The Contribution of the Cognitive Neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123-141. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747978
|
[93]
|
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The Double-Deficit Hypothesis for the Developmental Dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415
|
[94]
|
Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading Fluency and Its Intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211-239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2
|
[95]
|
Zayed, A. M., Roehrig, A. D., Arrastia-Lloyd, C. M., & Gilgil, N. M. (2013). Phonological Awareness and Working Memory in Arabic-Speaking Egyptian Preschool Children at Risk for Dyslexia. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 5, 139-149.
|
[96]
|
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading Acquisition, Developmental Dyslexia, and Skilled Reading across Languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size Theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
|
[97]
|
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Korne, G. (2003). Developmental Dyslexia Indifferent Languages: Language-Specific or Universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169-193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00139-5
|
[98]
|
Zeigler, J., Pech-Georgel, C., Dufau, S., & Grainger, J. (2010). Rapid Processing of Letters, Digits and Symbols: What Purely Visual-Attentional Deficit in Developmental Dyslexia? Developmental Science, 13, F8-F14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00983.x
|