Executive and Non-Executive Cognitive Abilities in Teenagers: Differences as a Function of Intelligence

Abstract

Intelligence and cognitive abilities, including executive functions (EF), have been addressed by psychometrics and cognitive psychology, respectively. Studies have found similarities and overlap among constructs, especially between EF and fluid intelligence (Gf). This study’s aim was to investigate in teenagers: 1) the relationships among Gf, crystallized intelligence (Gc), cognitive, and executive abilities; and 2) the differences among groups with average, superior and very superior intelligence in regard to cognitive and executive functions. A total of 120 adolescents aged between 15 and 16 years old were assessed via IQ tests (the WISC III and Raven’s), EF (computer version of the Stroop Test, FAS Verbal Fluency Test, Trail Making Test—part B), and cognitive abilities (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [PPVT], Repetition of words and pseudo words Test, the Rey Complex Figure [REY CF]). Low to moderate correlations were found among measures of intelligence and cognitive and executive functions. Even though interrelated, the measures seem to capture somewhat distinct aspects. Subsequently, the participants were divided into three groups according to their performance on Raven’s Test: Group with very superior intelligence (VSI), Group with superior intelligence (SI), and Group with average intelligence (AI). The ANOVA revealed the groups’ significant effect (VSI, SI, AI), that is, the VSI and SI groups tended to perform better on the WISC subtests, in the cognitive measures of the PPVT, Rey CF, and in executive measure (FAS). A tendency of increasingly better performance in the various abilities according to groups was observed, but the hypothesis of greater specific association between Gf and EF was not confirmed. The results show better general performance according to the level of intelligence.

Share and Cite:

Godoy, S. , Dias, N. & Seabra, A. (2014). Executive and Non-Executive Cognitive Abilities in Teenagers: Differences as a Function of Intelligence. Psychology, 5, 2018-2032. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.518205.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Abreu, N., Siquara, G. M., Conceicao, A. F. S., Leahy, I., Nikaedo, C., & Engel, P. (2014). Relacoes entre Inteligencia e Funcoes Executivas. In A. G. Seabra, J. A. Laros, E. C. de Macedo, & N. Abreu (Eds.), Inteligencia e Funcoes Executivas (pp. 51-71). Sao Paulo: Memnon Edicoes Cientificas.
[2] Angelini, A. L., Alves, I. C. B., Custodio, E. M., Duarte, W. F., & Duarte, J. L. M. (1999). Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala Especial. Manual. Sao Paulo: CETEPP.
[3] Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 31-36.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/15.1.31
[4] Arffa, S. (2007). The Relationship of Intelligence to Executive Function and Non-Executive Function Measures in Sample of Average, above Average and Gifted Youth. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 969-978.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.001
[5] Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., & Grafman, J. (2012). An Integrative Architecture for General Intelligence and Executive Function Revealed by Lesion Mapping. Brain: A journal of Neurology, 135, 1154-1164.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws021
[6] Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312
[7] Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its Structure, Growth and Action. New York: North-Holland.
[8] Cattell, R. B. (1998). Where Is Intelligence? Some Answers from the Triadic Theory. In J. Mcardle, & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human Cognitive Abilities in Theory and Practice (pp. 29-38). New Jersey: Erlbaum.
[9] Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., Shayer, M., van der Ven, S., Brydges, C. R., Kroesbergen, E., Podjarny, G., & Swanson, H. L. (2014). Relations between Speed, Working Memory, and Intelligence from Preschool to Adulthood: Structural Equation Modeling of 14 Studies. Intelligence, 46, 107-121.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.013
[10] Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
[11] Dias, N. M., & Seabra, A. G. (2014). The FAS Fluency Test in Brazilian Children and Teenagers: Executive Demands and the Effects of Age and Gender. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria (Impresso), 72, 55-62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282X20130213
[12] Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
[13] Figueiredo, V. L. (2002). Wisc III. In J. Cunha (Ed.), Psicodiagnostico V (pp. 603-614). Porto Alegre: Artmed.
[14] Floyd, R. G., Bergeron, R., Hamilton, G., & Parra, G. R. (2010). How Do Executive Functions Fit with the Cattell-Horn-Carroll Model? Some Evidence from a Joint Factor Analysis of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System and the Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Psychology in the Schools, 47, 721-738.
[15] Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not All Executive Functions Are Related to Intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172-179.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x
[16] Furgueson, C., Greenstein, D., Mcguffin, P., & Soffer, S. (1999). Efficacy of the WISC-III Short Form for Children Diagnosed with Conduct and Oppositional Defiant Disorders. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 6-7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/14.1.6
[17] Garderen, D. V. (2006). Spatial Visualization, Visual Imagery, and Mathematical Problem Solving of Students with Varying Abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 496-506.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194060390060201
[18] Gustafsson, J. E. (1988). Hierarchical Models of Individual Differences in Cognitive Abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence (pp. 35-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[19] Harnqvist, K., Gustafsson, J. E., Muthen, B. O., & Nelson, G. (1994). Hierarchical Models of Ability at Individuals and Class Levels. Intelligence, 18, 165-187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(94)90026-4
[20] Horn, J. L. (1991). Measurement of Intellectual Capabilities: A Review of Theory. In K. S. Mcgrew, J. K. Werder, & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Woodcock-Johnson Technical Manual (pp. 197-232). Chicago, IL: Riverside.
[21] Horn, J. L., & Noll, J. (1997). Human Cognitive Capabilities: Gf-Gc Theory. In D. Flanagan, J. Genshaft, & P. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues (pp. 53-91). New York: Guilford.
[22] Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
[23] Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Sedo, M., Fuentes, D., & Leite, W. B. (2008). Neuropsicologia das funcoes ejecutivas. In D. Fuentes, L. F. Malloy-Diniz, C. H. P. Camargo, & R. M. Cosenza (Eds.), Neuropsicologia: Teoria e Pratica (pp. 187-206). Porto Alegre: Artmed.
[24] Mcgrew, K. S. (1997). Analysis of the Major Intelligence Batteries According to a Proposed Comprehensive Gf-Gc Framework. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues (pp. 151-179). New York: Guilford.
[25] Mcgrew, K. S. (2009). CHC Theory and the Human Cognitive Abilities Project: Standing on the Shoulders of the Giants of Psychometric Intelligence Research. Intelligence, 37, 1-10.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004
[26] Mcgrew, K. S., & Flanagan, D. P. (1998). The Intelligence Test Desk Reference (ITDR): Gf-Gc Cross-Battery Assessment. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
[27] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
[28] Montiel, J., & Seabra, A. G. (2012). Teste de Trilhas. In A. G. Seabra, & N. M. Dias (Eds.), Avaliacao neuropsicologica cognitiva: Atencao e funcoes executivas (pp. 69-75). Sao Paulo: Memnon.
[29] Primi, R. (2003). Inteligencia: Avancos nos modelos teoricos e nos instrumentos de Medida. Avaliacao Psicologica, 2, 67-77.
[30] Rey, A. (1999). Teste de copia e de reproducao de memoria de figuras complexas geometricas complexas: Manual. Sao Paulo: Casa do Psicologo.
[31] Riccio, C. A., Hall, J., Morgan, A., & Hynd, G. W. (1994). Executive Function and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Relationship with Behavioral Ratings and Cognitive Ability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10, 215-229.
[32] Roca, M., Parr, A., Thompson, R., Woolgar, A., Torralva, T., Antoun, N., Manes, F., & Duncan, J. (2010). Executive Function and Fluid Intelligence after Frontal Lobe Lesions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 133, 234-247.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp269
[33] Schelini, P. W. (2006). Teoria das inteligencias fluida e cristalizada: Inicio e evolucao. Estudos de Psicologia, 11, 323-332.
[34] Seabra, A. G. (2012). Teste de Repeticao de Palavras e Pseudopalavras. In A. G. Seabra, N. M. Dias, & F. C. Capovilla (Orgs.), Avaliacao Neuropsicologica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral, Vol. 2, Sao Paulo: Memnon.
[35] Seabra, A. G., Dias, N. M., & Macedo, E. C. (in press). Teste de Stroop Computadorizado. Sao Paulo: Memnon.
[36] Simoes, M. R. (2002). Utilizacoes da WISC-III na avaliacao neuropsicologica de criancas e adolescentes. Paideia, 12, 113-132.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0103-863X2002000200009
[37] Sternberg, R. J. (2008). Psicologia Cognitiva (4th ed.). Porto Alegre: Artmed.
[38] Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.
[39] Sullivan, J. R., Riccio, C. A., & Castillo, C. R. (2009). Concurrent Validity of the Tower Tasks as Measures of Executive Function in Adults: A Meta-Analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 62-75.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09084280802644243
[40] Van Aken, L., Kessels, R. P. C., Wingbermühle, E. Wiltink, M., van der Heijden, P. T., & Egger, J. I. M. (2014). Exploring the Incorporation of Executive Functions in Intelligence Testing: Factor Analysis of the WAIS-III and Traditional Tasks of Executive Functioning. International Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 73-80.
[41] Vance, M. (2004). Avaliacao da habilidade de processamento da fala nas criancas: uma analise de tarefas. In M. Snowling, & J. Stackhouse (Eds.), Dislexia, fala e linguagem (pp. 57-73). Porto Alegre: Artes Medicas.
[42] Zook, N. A., Davalos, D. B., Delosh, E. L., & Davis, H. P. (2004). Working Memory, Inhibition, and Fluid Intelligence as Predictors of Performance on Tower of Hanoi and London Tasks. Brain and Cognition, 56, 286-292.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.07.003

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.