Conscientiousness Moderates the Influence of a Help-Eliciting Prime on Prosocial Behavior

Abstract

This study tested whether personality influenced the effect of a help-eliciting prime. Participants (N = 203) attended two experimental sessions. In the first session, Conscientiousness was measured along with several control variables. Participants returned for a second session where they were randomly assigned to a help-eliciting prime or a Control prime. After completing the written prime exercise, participants were asked to respond to a helping scenario. Textual analyses showed that written responses to the help-eliciting prime differed across level of Conscientiousness, with individuals high in Conscientiousness writing more often about one’s moral obligation to help. An ANCOVA analysis revealed a significant main effect for Conscientiousness, as well as a significant interaction effect between Conscientiousness and the Prime. Individuals scoring higher in Conscientiousness were more likely to provide assistance, and this was particularly true if they were in the help-eliciting prime condition. Our results are supportive of an interactional model where one must consider both personality and situational factors to successfully understand and predict helping responses.

Share and Cite:

Swickert, R. , Abushanab, B. , Bise, H. & Szer, R. (2014). Conscientiousness Moderates the Influence of a Help-Eliciting Prime on Prosocial Behavior. Psychology, 5, 1954-1961. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.517198.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Allemand, M., Zimprich, D., & Hertwog, C. (2007). Cross-Sectional Age Differences and Longitudinal Age Changes of Personality in Middle Adulthood and Old Age. Journal of Personality, 75, 323-358.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00441.x
[2] Bargh, J., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230-244.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
[3] Blass, T. (1991). Understanding Behavior in the Milgram Obedience Experiment: The Role of Personality, Situations, and Their Interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 398-413.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.398
[4] Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
[5] Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., & Lepinasse, V. (2001). Reflecting and Deflecting Stereotypes: Assimilation and Contrast in Impression Formation and Automatic Behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 286-299.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2000.1449
[6] Dijksterhuis, A., Spears, R., Postmes, T., Stapel, K., Koomen, W., van Knippenberg, A., & Scheepers, D. (1998). Seeing One Thing and Doing Another: Contrast Effects in Automatic Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 862-871.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.862
[7] Ekehammer, B. (1974). Interactionism in Personality from a Historical Perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1026-1048.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0037457
[8] Endler, N., & Magnusson, D. (1976). Interactional Psychology and Personality. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
[9] Ferguson, E. (2004). Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, Perceived Control and the Frequency, Recent, Rate and Years of Blood Donor Behavior. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 293-314.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/1359107041557011
[10] Kawakami, K., Young, H., & Dovidio, J. (2012). Automatic Stereotyping: Category, Trait, and Behavioral Activations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3-15.
[11] Ladd, D., & Henry, R. (2000). Helping Coworkers and Helping the Organization: The Role of Support Perceptions, Exchange Ideology, and Conscientiousness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2028-2049.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02422.x
[12] McCrae, R., & Costa, P. (1989). The NEO Five Factor Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
[13] Meier, B., Robinson, M., & Wilkowski, B. (2006). Turning the Other Cheek: Agreeableness and the Regulation of Aggression-Related Primes. Psychological Science, 17, 136-142.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01676.x
[14] Moeller, S., Robinson, M., & Bresin, K. (2010). Integrating Trait and Social-Cognitive Views of Personality: Neuroticism, Implicit Stress Priming, and Neuroticism-Outcome Relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 677-689.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167210367487
[15] Moon, H., Livne, E., & Marinova, S. (2013). Understanding the Independent Influence of Duty and Achievement Striving When Predicting the Relationship between Conscientiousness and Organizational Cultural Profiles and Helping Behaviors. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 225-232.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.740539
[16] Nelson, L., & Norton, M. (2005). From Student to Superhero: Situational Primes Shape Future Helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 423-430.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.003
[17] Organ, D., & Ryan, K. (1995). A Meta-Analytic Review of Attitudinal and Dispositional Predictors of Organizational Citizenship Behavior. Personnel Psychology, 35, 1-62.
[18] Pittinsky, T., Shih, M., & Ambady, N. (2000). Will a Category Cue Affect you? Category Cues, Positive Stereotypes and Reviewer Recall for Applicants. Social Psychology of Education, 4, 53-65.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009656413789
[19] Reynolds, W. (1982). Development of Reliable and Valid Short Forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 119-125.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119::AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-I
[20] Robinson, M., Moeller, S., & Ode, S. (2010). Extraversion and Reward-Related Processing: Probing Incentive Motivation in Affective Priming Tasks. Emotion, 10, 615-626.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019173
[21] Soldz, S., & Vaillant, G. E. (1999). The Big Five Personality Traits and the Life Course: A 45-Year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 208-232.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1999.2243
[22] Stapel, D., Koomen, W., & van der Plight, J. (1996). The Referents of Trait Inferences: The Impact of Trait Concepts versus Actor-Trail Links on Subsequent Judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 437-450.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.437
[23] Steele, C., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
[24] vanEmmerik, I., & Euwema, M. (2007). Who Is Offering a Helping Hand? Associations between Personality and OCBs, and the Moderating Role of Team Leader Effectiveness. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22, 530-548.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683940710778422
[25] Wheeler, S., DeMarree, K., & Petty, R. (2007). Understanding the Role of the Self in Prime-to-Behavior Effects: The Active-Self Account. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 234-261.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868307302223

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.