[1]
|
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Priming on Action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230-244 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230
|
[2]
|
Bargh, J. A., & Shalev, I. (2011). The Substitutability of Physical and Social Warmth in Daily Life. Emotion, 12, 154-162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023527
|
[3]
|
Bilalic, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2010). Inflexibility of Experts—Reality or Myth? Quantifying the Einstellung Effect in Chess Masters. Cognitive Psychology, 56, 73-102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.02.001
|
[4]
|
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in Chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2
|
[5]
|
Chassy, P. & Gobet, F. (2011). A Hypothesis about the Biological Basis of Expert Intuition. Review of General Psychology, 15, 198-212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023958
|
[6]
|
de Groot, A. (1946). Het denken van den Schaker. Amsterdam: Noord Hollandsche
|
[7]
|
de Groot, A. (1965). Thought and Choice in Chess (2nd ed.). The Hague: Mouton Publishers.
|
[8]
|
de Groot, A. & Gobet, F. (1996). Perception and Memory in Chess. Heuristics of the Professional Eye. Assen: Van Gorcum.
|
[9]
|
Elo, A. (1978). The Rating of Chessplayers, Past and Present. New York: Arco.
|
[10]
|
Erb, H. P., Bioy, A., & Hilton, D. J. (2002). Choice Preferences without Inferences: Subconscious Priming of Risk Attitudes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5, 250-258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.416
|
[11]
|
Forster, J., & Liberman, N. (2007). Knowledge Activation. In A. Kruglanski, & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed., pp. 201-231). New York: Guilford Press.
|
[12]
|
Freud, S. (1901). The Psychopathology of Everyday Life: Forgetting, Slips of the Tongue, Bungled Actions, Superstitions and Errors. London: Ernest Benn Limited.
|
[13]
|
Freud, S. (1920). A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis. New York: Horace Liveright. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10667-000
|
[14]
|
Gerdes, C., & Gransmark, P. (2010). Strategic Behavior across Gender: A Comparison of Female and Male Expert Chess Players. Labour Economics, 17, 766-775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2010.04.013
|
[15]
|
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). The Roles of Recognition Processes and Look ahead Search in Time-Constrained Expert Problem Solving: Evidence from Grandmaster Level Chess. Psychological Science, 7, 52-55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00666.x
|
[16]
|
Gobet, F., de Voogt, A., & Retschitzki, J. (2004). Moves in Mind: The Psychology of Board Games. Hove: Psychology Press.
|
[17]
|
Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2007). The Role of Domain-Specific Practice, Handedness and Starting Age in Chess. Developmental Psychology, 43, 159-172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.159
|
[18]
|
Hassin, R. R., Ferguson, M. J., Shidlovsky, D., & Gross, T. (2007). Subliminal Exposure to National Flags Affects Political Thought and Behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19757-19761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704679104
|
[19]
|
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Zimerman, S. (2009). Automatic and Flexible: The Case of Nonconscious Goal Pursuit. Social Cognition, 27, 20-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.1.20
|
[20]
|
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material Priming: The Influence of Mundane Physical Objects on Situational Construal and Competitive Behavioral Choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 83-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.06.003
|
[21]
|
Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Pohl, C., Berner, M. P., & Hoffmann, J. (2009) Playing Chess Unconsciously. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 292-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014499
|
[22]
|
Mussweiler, T., & Forster, J. (2000). The Sex-Aggression Link: A Perception-Behavior Dissociation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 507-520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.507
|
[23]
|
Reingold, E. M., Charness, N., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D. M. (2001). Visual Span in Expert Chess Players: Evidence from Eye Movements. Psychological Science, 12, 49-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00309
|
[24]
|
Saariluoma, P. (1991). Aspects of Skilled Imagery in Blindfold Chess. Acta Psychologica, 77, 65-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(91)90065-8
|
[25]
|
Shanteau, J. (1992). Competence in Experts: The Role of Task Characteristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53, 252-266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(92)90064-E
|
[26]
|
Shantz, A., & Latham, G. P. (2009). An Exploratory Field Experiment of the Effect of Subconscious and Conscious Goals on Employee Performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 9-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.01.001
|
[27]
|
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing. II. Perceptual Learning, Automatic Attending and a General Theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127-190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
|
[28]
|
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in Chess. American Scientist, 61, 394-403.
|
[29]
|
Weiss, D. J., & Shanteau, J. (2003). Empirical Assessment of Expertise. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 45, 104-116.
|