Self-Motion Perception Induced by Cutaneous Sensation Caused by Constant Wind

Abstract

We first in the history of vection research, challenged to induce cutaneous vection by providing the participants body with wind. Participants wore an eye mask to block out all outside visual information, and white noise was presented through a pair of earphones to block out all outside auditory information. We also provided body sway by using a horse-riding machine. The constant wind was provided to the participants from the front, side and behind of them. The results clearly showed that the cutaneous vection by wind was obtained in almost all participants. Even by only wind to the body could induce vection.

Share and Cite:

Murata, K. , Seno, T. , Ozawa, Y. & Ichihara, S. (2014). Self-Motion Perception Induced by Cutaneous Sensation Caused by Constant Wind. Psychology, 5, 1777-1782. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.515184.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Allison, R. S., Zacher, J. E., Kirollos, R., Guterman, P. S., & Palmisano, S. (2012). Perception of Smooth and Perturbed Vection in Short-Duration Microgravity. Experimental Brain Research, 223, 479-487.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3275-5
[2] Andersen, G. J. (1986). Perception of Self-Motion: Psychophysical and Computational Approaches. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 52-65.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.52
[3] Ash, A., Palmisano, S., Apthorp, D., & Allisom, R. S. (2013). Vection in Depth during Treadmill Walking. Perception, 42, 562-576.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7449
[4] Benson, A. J. (1990). Sensory Functions and Limitations of the Vestibular System. In R. Warren & A. H. Wertheim (Eds.), The Perception and Control of Self-Motion (pp. 145-170), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[5] Berthoz, A., & Droulez, J. (1982). Linear Self-Motion Perception. In A. H. Wertheim, W. A. Wagenaar & H. W. Leibowitz (Eds.), Tutorials on Motion Perception (pp. 157-199), London: Plenum.
[6] Brandt, T., Wist, E. R., & Dichgans, J. (1975). Foreground and Background in Dynamic Spatial Orientation. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 497-503.
[7] Dichgans, J., Bizzi, E., Morasso, P., & Tagliasco, V. (1974). The Role of Vestibular and Neck Afferents During Eye-Head Coordination in the Monkey. Brain Research, 71, 225-232.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(74)90964-0
[8] Feuereissen, D. (2008). VR: Getting the Reality Part Straight—Does Jitter and Suspension of the Human Body Increase Auditory Circular Vection? Bachelor’s Thesis, Department of Computer Science in Media.
[9] Fischer, M. H., & Kornmüller, A. E. (1930). Optokinetischausgeloste Bewegungswahrnehmung und optokinetischer Nystagmus. Journal fur Psychologie und Neurologie (Leipzig), 41, 273-308.
[10] Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
[11] Howard, I. P. (1986). The Perception of Posture, Self Motion and the Visual Vertical. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Perception (pp. 18-1-18-62). New York: Wiley & Sons.
[12] Johansson, G. (1977). Studies on Visual-Perception of Locomotion. Perception, 6, 365-376.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p060365
[13] Kano, C. (1991). The Perception of Self-Motion Induced by Peripheral Visual Information in Sitting and Supine Postures. Ecological Psychology, 3, 241-253.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco0303_3
[14] Lackner, J. R. (1977). Induction of Illusory Self-Motion and Nystagmus by a Rotating Sound-Field. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 48, 129-131.
[15] Lee, D. N., & Aronson, E. (1974). Visual Proprioceptive Control of Standing in Human Infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 529-532.
[16] Lestienne, F., Soechting, J., & Berthoz, A. (1977). Postural Readjustments Induced by Linear Motion of Visual Scenes. Experimental Brain Research, 28, 363-384.
[17] Lishman, J. R., and Lee, D. N. (1973). The Autonomy of Visual Kinaesthesis. Perception, 2, 287-294.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p020287
[18] Mueller, C. H., Kornilova, L., Wiest, G., & Steinhoff, N. (1994). Psychophysical Studies of Visuo-Vestibular Interaction in Microgravity. Acta Astronautica, 33, 9-13.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-5765(94)90102-3
[19] Mutaguchi, T. (2012). A Study on the Development of Orientation and Mobility Instruction Program for the Blind Children. In T. Mutaguchi (Principal Investigator), 2008-2011 Ministry of Education, Science and Culture Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) Research Report.
[20] Nakamura, S., & Shimojo, S. (1998). Orientation of Selective Effects of Body Tilt on Visually Induced Perception of Self-Motion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 667-672.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.2.667
[21] Riecke, B. E., Schulte-Pelkum, J., Caniard, F., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2005). Influence of Auditory Cues on the Visually-Induced Self-Motion Illusion (Circular Vection) in Virtual Reality. In Proceedings of 8th Annual Workshop Presence 2005 (pp. 49-57). New York: ACM Press.
[22] Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of Human Locomotion and Models of Perceptual-Motor Organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480-497.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.480
[23] Sakamoto, S., Osada, Y., Suzuki, Y., & Gyoba, J. (2004).The Effects of Linearly Moving Sound Images on Self-Motion Perception. Acoustical Science and Technology, 25, 100-102.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1250/ast.25.100
[24] Sakurai, K., Kubodera, T., & Grove, P. (2010). Multi-Modally Perceived Direction of Self-Motion from Orthogonally Directed Visual and Vestibular Stimulation. Journal of Vision, 10, 866.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/10.7.866
[25] Seno, T., Funatsu, F., & Palmisano, S. (2013). Virtual Swimming-Breaststroke Body Movements Facilitate Vection. Multisensory Research, 6, 267-275.
[26] Seno, T., Hasuo, E., Ito, H., & Nakajima, Y. (2012). Perceptually Plausible Sounds Facilitate Visually Induced Self-Motion Perception (Vection). Perception, 41, 577-593.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7184
[27] Seno, T., Ito, H., & Sunaga, S. (2011). Inconsistent Locomotion Inhibits Vection. Perception, 40, 747-750.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7018
[28] Seno, T., Ogawa, M., Ito, H., & Sunaga, S. (2011). Consistent Air Flow to the Face Facilitates Vection. Perception, 40, 1237-1240.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7055
[29] Siegler, I., Viaud-Delmon, I., Israel, I., & Berthoz, A. (2000). Self-Motion Perception during a Sequence of Whole-Body Rotations in Darkness. Experimental Brain Research, 134, 66-73.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002210000415
[30] Valjamae, A. (2009). Auditorily-Induced Illusory Self-Motion: A Review. Brain Research Review, 61, 240-255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.07.001
[31] Wong, S. C. P., & Frost, B. J. (1981). The Effect of Visual-Vestibular Conflict on the Latency of Steady-State Visually Induced Subjective Rotation. Perception & Psychophysics, 30, 228-236.
[32] Young, L. R., & Shelhamer, M. (1990). Microgravity Enhances the Relative Contribution of Visually-Induced Motion Sensation. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 61, 525-530.

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.