Relationship between Video Game Violence and Long-Term Neuropsychological Outcomes

Abstract

The current study examined the long-term effects of video game violence on aggressiveness and facial expression recognition using multiple measures. In Experiment 1, participants unfamiliar with video games were randomly assigned to play a violent or nonviolent video game for four weeks. Before and after the game play interval, event-related potentials (ERP) evoked by facial expressions were recorded, and aggressiveness was measured with a questionnaire. Results showed that playing a violent video game delayed peak latency of a positive component of the ERP evoked by angry faces and increased aggressiveness among male participants. Experiment 2 included a 3-month follow-up assessment. Results showed preservation of delayed neural activity, while levels of aggressiveness diminished to some extent. These findings highlight differential aspects regarding the long-term effects of playing a violent video game: more enduring for facial expression recognition and short-lived for aggressiveness.

Share and Cite:

Tamamiya, Y. , Matsuda, G. & Hiraki, K. (2014). Relationship between Video Game Violence and Long-Term Neuropsychological Outcomes. Psychology, 5, 1477-1487. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.513159.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Anderson, C. A., Berkowitz, L., Donnerstein, E., Huesmann, R. L., Johnson, J., Linz, D., &Wartella, E. (2003). The Influence of Media Violence on Youth. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4, 81-110.
[2] Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video Games and Aggressive Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior in the Laboratory and in Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772-790.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.772
[3] Anderson, C. A., Sakamoto, A., Gentile, D. A., Ihori, N., Shibuya, A., Yukawa, S., & Kobayashi, K. (2008). Longitudinal Effects of Violent Video Games on Aggression in Japan and the United States. Pediatrics, 122, e1067-e1072.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1425
[4] Anderson, C. A, Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). Violent Video Game Effects on Aggression, Empathy, and Prosocial Behavior in Eastern and Western Countries: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151-173.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018251
[5] Ando, A., Soga, S., Yamasaki, K., Shimai, S., Shimada, H., Utsuki, N., & Sakai, A. (1999). Development of the Japanese Version of the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BAQ). The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 70, 384-392. (in Japanese).
http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.70.384
[6] Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Video Game Training Enhances Cognitive Control in Older Adults. Nature, 501, 97-101.
[7] Bailey, K., West, R., & Anderson, C. A. (2011). The Association between Chronic Exposure to Video Game Violence and Affective Picture Processing: An ERP Study. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 259-276.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0029-y
[8] Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic Violent Video Game Exposure and Desensitization to Violence: Behavioral and Event-Related Brain Potential Data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 532-539.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.006
[9] Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 551-565.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551
[10] Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002). Violent Video Games and Hostile Expectations: A Test of the General Aggression Model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 78, 1679-1686.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616702237649
[11] Bushman, B. J., & Gibson, B. (2011). Violent Video Games Cause an Increase in Aggression Long after the Game Has Been Turned off. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 29-32.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550610379506
[12] Carnagey, N. L., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). The Effect of Video Game Violence on Physiological Desensitization to Real-Life Violence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 489-496.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.003
[13] Devilly, G. J., Callahan, P., & Armitage, G. (2012). The Effect of Violent Videogame Playtime on Anger. Australian Psychologist, 47, 98-107.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1742-9544.2010.00008.x
[14] Eckhardt, C. I., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Attention to Anger-Relevant and Irrelevant Stimuli Following Naturalistic Insult. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 619-629.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00074-3
[15] Engelhardt, C. R., Bartholow, B. D., Kerr, G. T., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). This Is Your Brain on Violent Video Games: Neural Desensitization to Violence Predicts Increased Aggression Following Violent Video Game Exposure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1033-1036.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.027
[16] Freedman, J. L. (1984). Effects of Television Violence on Aggressiveness. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 227-246.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.96.2.227
[17] Funk, J. B., Daldacci, H. B., Pasold, T., & Baumgardner, J. (2004). Violence Exposure in Real-Life, Video Games, Television, Movies, and the Internet: Is There Desensitization? Journal of Adolescence, 27, 23-39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.005
[18] Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. L., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The Effects of Violent Video Game Habits on Adolescent Hostility, Aggressive Behaviors, and School Performance. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 5-22.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.002
[19] Green, C. S., Li, R., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Perceptual Learning during Action Video Game Playing. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 202-216.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01054.x
[20] Hasan, Y., Bègue, L., Scharkow, M., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). The More You Play, the More Aggressive You Become: A Long-Term Experimental Study of Cumulative Violent Video Game Effects on Hostile Expectations and Aggressive Behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 224-227.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.016
[21] Horovitz, S. G., Rossion, B., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J. C. (2004). Parametric Design and Correlational Analyses Help Integrating fMRI and Electrophysiological Data during Face Processing. NeuroImage, 22, 1587-1595.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.018
[22] Ihori, N., Sakamoto, A., Kobayashi, K., & Kimura, H. (2003). A Panel Study on Causal Relationships between Video Game Use and Aggressiveness in Elementary School Children: Effects on Physical Violence. Simulation & Gaming, 13, 139-148. (in Japanese)
[23] Jemel, B., George, N., Chaby, L., Fiori, N., & Renault, B. (1999). Differential Processing of Part-to-Whole and Part-to-Part Face Priming: An ERP Study. NeuroReport, 10, 1069-1075.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199904060-00031
[24] Jeon, S. T., Lewis, T. L., & Maurer, D. (2012). The Effect of Video Game Training on the Vision of Adults with Bilateral Deprivation Amblyopia. Seeing and Perceiving, 25, 493-520.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/18784763-00002391
[25] Kirsh, S. J., & Mounts, J. R. W. (2007). Violent Video Game Play Impacts Facial Emotion Recognition. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 353-358.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20191
[26] Kirsh, S. J., Mounts, J. R. W., & Olczak, P. V. (2006). Violent Media Consumption and the Recognition of Dynamic Facial Expressions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 571-584.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260506286840
[27] Kirsh, S. J., Olczak, P. V., & Mounts, J. R. W. (2005). Violent Video Games Induce an Affect Processing Bias. Media Psychology, 7, 239-250.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0703_1
[28] Kuhn, S., Gleich, T., Lorenz, R. C., Lindenberger, U., & Gallinat, J. (2014). Playing Super Mario Induces Structural Brain Plasticity: Gray Matter Changes Resulting from Training with a Commercial Video Game. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, 265-271.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/mp.2013.120
[29] Lang, A. (2006). Using the Limited Capacity Model of Motivated Mediated Message Processing to Design Effective Cancer Communication Messages. Journal of Communication, 56, S57-S80.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00283.x
[30] Latinus, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2005). Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1316-1327.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/0898929055002490
[31] Sharma, D., & McKenna, F. P. (2001). The Role of Time Pressure on the Emotional Stroop Task. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 471-481.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712601162293
[32] Staude-Muller, F., Bliesener, T., & Luthman, S. (2008). Hostile and Hardened? An Experimental Study on (De-)Sensitization to Violence and Suffering through Playing Video Games. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 41-50.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.67.1.41
[33] Tamamiya, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2013). Individual Differences in the Recognition of Facial Expressions: An Event-Related Potentials Study. PLoS ONE, 8, Article ID: e57325.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057325
[34] Wallenius, M., & Punamaki, R. (2008). Digital Game Violence and Direct Aggression in Adolescence: A Longitudinal Study of the Roles of Sex, Age, and Parent-Child Communication. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 286-294.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.04.010
[35] Weber, R., Ritterfeld, U., & Mathiak, K. (2006). Does Playing Violent Video Games Induce Aggression? Empirical Evidence of a Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Media Psychology, 8, 39-60.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0801_4

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.