Effects of Emotional Context during Encoding: An Advantage for Negative Context in Immediate Recognition and Positive Context in Delayed Recognition

Abstract

This study examined the effect of emotional context on recognition memory for neutral targets over time. Participants (N = 36) read sentences with negative, neutral, or positive valence in which emotionally neutral target words were embedded. Two groups of participants were asked to recognize targets after different retention intervals, immediate and 24-hour. Performance was better in the immediate recognition group than that in the delayed recognition group. The two groups showed opposite effects of emotional context: Target words in negative sentences were best remembered with immediate recognition but worst after a 24-hour delay. These results support the idea that emotional valence has different impacts on memory over time.

Share and Cite:

Toyama, A. , Katsuhara, M. , Sakurai, Y. & Ohira, H. (2014). Effects of Emotional Context during Encoding: An Advantage for Negative Context in Immediate Recognition and Positive Context in Delayed Recognition. Psychology, 5, 994-1000. doi: 10.4236/psych.2014.59111.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. (1994). Affect and Memory in Retrospective Reports. In N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Autobiographical Memory and the Validity of Retrospective Reports (pp. 71-86). New York: Springer-Verlag.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2624-6_6
[2] Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad Is Stronger than Good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323
[3] Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary Autobiographical Memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 435-454.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199610)10:5<435::AID-ACP408>3.0.CO;2-L
[4] De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative Learning of Likes and Dislikes: A Review of 25 Years of Research on Human Evaluative Conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853-869.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.853
[5] Dougal, S., & Rotello, C. M. (2007). “Remembering” Emotional Words Is Based on Response Bias, Not Recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 423-429.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194083
[6] Gibbons, J. A., Lee, S. A., & Walker, W. R. (2011). The Fading Affect Bias Begins within 12 Hours and Persists for 3 Months. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 663-672.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1738
[7] Gotoh, F., & Ohta, N. (2001). Affective Valence of Two-Compound Kanji Words. Tsukuba Psychology Research, 23, 45-52.
[8] Holmes, D. S. (1970). Differential Change in Affective Intensity and the Forgetting of Unpleasant Personal Experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 234-239.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029394
[9] Lewis, P. A., Cairney, S., Manning, L., & Critchley, H. D. (2011). The Impact of Overnight Consolidation upon Memory for Emotional and Neutral Encoding Contexts. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2619-2629.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.009
[10] Osak, M., Nishizaki, Y., Komori, M., & Osaka, N. (2002). Effect of Focus on Verbal Working Memory: Critical Role of the Focus Word in Reading. Memory & Cognition, 30, 562-571.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194957
[11] Phelps, E. A., LaBar, K. S., & Spencer, D. D. (1997). Memory for Emotional Words Following Unilateral Temporal Lobectomy. Brain and Cognition, 35, 85-109.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1997.0929
[12] Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic Vigilance: The Attention-Grabbing Power of Negative Social Information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
[13] Ritchie, T., Skowronski, J. J., Hartnett, J., Wells, B., & Walker, W. R. (2009). The Fading Affect Bias in the Context of Emotion Activation Level, Mood, and Personal Theories of Emotion Change. Memory, 17, 428-444.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210902791665
[14] Talarico, J. M., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Emotional Intensity Predicts Autobiographical Memory Experience. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1118-1132.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196886
[15] Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical Effects of Positive and Negative Events: The Mobilization-Minimization Hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.110.1.67
[16] Thompson, C. P. (1985). Memory for Unique Personal Events: Effects of Pleasantness. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 277-289.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00991832
[17] Wagenaar, W. A. (1986). My Memory: A Study of Autobiographical Memory over Six Years. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 225-252.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90013-7
[18] Wagner, U., Degirmenci, M., Drosopoulos, S., Perras, B., & Born, J. (2005). Effects of Cortisol Suppression on Sleep-Associated Consolidation of Neutral and Emotional Memory. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 885-893.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.008
[19] Waldfogel, S. (1948). The Frequency and Affective Character of Childhood Memories. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 62, i-39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093581
[20] Walker, W. R., & Skowronski, J. J. (2009). The Fading Affect Bias: But What the Hell Is It for? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1122-1136.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1614
[21] Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003). Life Is Pleasant—And Memory Helps to Keep It That Way! Review of General Psychology, 7, 203-210.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203
[22] Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (1997). Autobiographical Memory: Unpleasantness Fades Faster than Pleasantness over Time. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 399-413.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199710)11:5<399::AID-ACP462>3.0.CO;2-E

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.