Conditioned Taste Aversion Diminishes Sugar Intake

Abstract

Animals learn to avoid particular food when some of its sensory properties are associated with gastrointestinal discomfort. Twenty rats were exposed to free access to food and a sugar solution for ten days in first phase. During second phase, experimental group received a dose of LiCl. Control group received an injection without LiCl. Both groups had free access to a sugar solution and food restriction for three days. In the final phase, both groups returned to the conditions of first phase. Results showed a significant decrease in sugar intake after aversive conditioning regarding the intake registered in the initial phase in experimental group. Control group did not show any differences in its sugar intake before and after the experimental manipulation. The procedure carried out is discussed as a means to decrease sugar intake.

Share and Cite:

Moreno, A. , López-Espinoza, A. , López, I. , Santos, L. and Leonardo, F. (2014) Conditioned Taste Aversion Diminishes Sugar Intake. Food and Nutrition Sciences, 5, 997-1004. doi: 10.4236/fns.2014.511110.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Garb, J.L. and Stunkard, A.J. (1974) Taste Aversions in Man. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 131, 1204-1207.
[2] García, E. and Bach, L. (1999) Preferencias y Aversiones Alimentarias. Anuario de Psicología, 30, 55-77.
[3] Bernstein, I.L. (1991) Flavor aversion. In: Getchell, T.V., Doty, R.L., Bartoshuk, L.M. and Snow, J.B., Eds., Smell and Taste Health and Disease, Raven Press, New York, 417-428.
[4] Molero, A. (2007) Aprendizaje Aversivo Gustativo: Características, Paradigma y Mecanismos Cerebrales. Anales de Psicología, 23, 57-64.
[5] Ballesteros, M.A., Gallo, M. and Maldonado, A. (2001) Detección de aversiones gustativas inducidas por estímulos incondicionados débiles como la rotación. Psicológica, 22, 217-234.
[6] García, J. and Koelling, R.A. (1966) Relation of Cue to Consequence in Avoiding Learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123-124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03342209
[7] Schafe, G.E. and Bemstein, I.L. (1996) Taste Aversion Learning. In: Capaldi, E.D., Ed., Why We Eat What We Eat, American Psychological Association, Washington, 31-51.
[8] Díaz, E., De la Casa, L.G., Ruiz, G. and Baeyens, F. (2004) Aprendizaje sabor-sabor en la adquisición de preferencias gustativas. Psicológica, 25, 135-146.
[9] Garcia, J., Lasiter, P.S. and Bermúdez-Rattoni, F. (1985) A General Theory of Aversion Learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 443, 8-21.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb27060.x
[10] Schafe, G.E., Sollars, S.I. and Bemstein, I.L. (1995) The CS-US Interval in Taste Aversion Learning: A Brief Look. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 799-802.
[11] Eccles, S., Kim, E.M. and O’Hare, E. (2005) Granisetron Attenuates Exercise-Induced Conditioned Taste Aversion in the Rat. Appetite, 44, 325-328.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.02.001
[12] Masaki, T. and Nakajima, S. (2005) Further Evidence for Conditioned Taste Aversion Induced by Forced Swimming. Physiology & Behavior, 84, 9-15.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.09.022
[13] Goudie, A.J. (1979) Aversive Stimulus Properties of Drugs. Neuropharmacology, 18, 971-979.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3908(79)90161-8
[14] Parker, L.A., Hills, K. and Jensen, K. (1984) Behavioral CRs Elicited by a Lithiumor an Amphetamine-Paired Contextual Test Chamber. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 307-315.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199972
[15] Bernstein, I.L. and Borson, S. (1986) Learned Food Aversion: A Component of Anorexia Syndromes. Psychologiocal Review, 94, 462-472.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.462
[16] Bures, J. (1998) Ethology, Phiysiological Psychology, and Neurobiology of CTA. In: Bures, J., Bermúdez-Rattoni, F. and Yamamoto, T., Eds., Conditioned Taste Aversion: Memory of a Special Kind, Oxford University Press, New York, 14-25.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523475.003.0002
[17] Archer, T. and Sjodén, P. (1979) Positive Correlation between Preand Postconditioning Saccharin Intake in TasteAversion Learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 144-148.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209263
[18] Peck, J. and Ader, R. (1974) Illness-Induced Taste Aversion under States of Deprivation and Satiation. Animal Learning & Behavior, 2, 6-8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199107
[19] Nachman, M. and Ashe, J.H. (1973) Learned Taste Aversions in Rats as a Function of Dosage, Concentration, and Route of Administration of LiCl. Physiology & Behavior, 10, 73-78.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(73)90089-9
[20] Loy, I. and Hall, G. (2002) Taste Aversion after Ingestion of Lithium Chloride: An Associative Analysis. The Quarterly. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55b, 365-380.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724990244000070
[21] Ackroff, K. and Sclafani, A. (2004) Fructose-Conditioned Flavor Preferences in Male and Female Rats: Effects of Sweet Taste and Sugar Concentration. Appetite, 42, 287-297.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.05.059
[22] Ackroff, K., Manza, L. and Sclafani, A. (1993) The Rat’s Preferente for Sucrose, Polycose and Their Mixtures. Appetite, 21, 69-80.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1037
[23] Booth, D. (1990) Learned Role of Tastes in Eating Motivation. In: Capaldi, E.D. and Powley, T., Eds., Taste, Experience, and Feeding, American Psychological Association, Washington, 179-195.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10075-013
[24] Bolles, R.C., Hayward, L. and Crandall, C. (1981) Conditioned Taste Preferences Based on Caloric Density. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 59-69.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.7.1.59
[25] Carper, J. and Polliard, F. (1953) A Comparison of the Intake of Glucose and Saccharin Solutions in Conditions of Caloric Need. American Journal of Psychology, 66, 479-482.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1418243
[26] Martínez, A.G., López-Espinoza, A., Díaz, F. and Valdés, E. (2009) Consumo de soluciones endulzadas en ratas albinas: Sabor vs calorías. Psicothema, 21, 196-203.
[27] Sclafani, A. (1990) Nutritionally Based Leamed Flavor Preferences in Rats. In: Capaldi, E.D. and Powley, T., Eds., Taste, Experience, and Feeding, American Psychological Association, Washington, 139-156.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10075-010
[28] Houpt, K.A., Zahorik, D.M. and Swartzman-Andert, J.A. (1990) Taste Aversion Learning in Horses. Journal of Animal Science, 68, 2340-2344.
[29] Parker, L.A. and Mac Leod, K.B. (1994) Chin Rub CRs May Reflect Conditioned Sickness Elicited by a Lithium-Paired Sucrose Solution. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 40, 983-986.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(91)90115-I
[30] Martínez, A.G. and López-Espinoza, A. (2007) Efectos post-privación con dos alternativas energéticas en ratas. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 33, 43-60.
[31] Martínez, A.G., López-Espinoza, A. and Martínez, H. (2006) Efectos de modificar el contenido energético del agua sobre el peso corporal, consumo de agua, alimento y calorías en ratas. Universitas Psychologica, 5, 361-370.

Copyright © 2023 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.