[1]
|
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., & Lang, P. J. (2006). A Multi-Process Account of Startle Modulation during Affective Perception. Psychophysiology, 43, 486-497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00412.x
|
[2]
|
Carlson, N. R. (2012). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). London: Pearson.
|
[3]
|
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1988). Mood and Mundane: Relations between Daily Life Events and Self-Reported Mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 296-308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.296
|
[4]
|
Eich, E., Ng, J. T., Macaulay, D. P. A. D., Percy, A. D., & Grebneva, I. (2007). Combining Music with Thought to Change Mood. Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, 124-136.
|
[5]
|
Eysenck, M. W. (1993). Principles of Cognitive Psychology. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
|
[6]
|
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global versus Local Processing of Visual Information. Psychological Science, 13, 34-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00406
|
[7]
|
Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar Expectancy Theory and Weber’s Law in Animal Timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279-325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.279
|
[8]
|
Gibbon, J. (1991). Origins of Scalar Timing. Learning and Motivation, 22, 3-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(91)90015-Z
|
[9]
|
Goldstone, S., & Lhamon, W. T. (1974). Studies of Auditory-Visual Differences in Human Time Judgment: I. Sounds are Judged Longer than Lights. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 63-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1974.39.1.63
|
[10]
|
Huber, F., Beckmann, S. C., & Herrmann, A. (2004). Means-End Analysis: Does the Affective State Influence Information Processing Style? Psychology & Marketing, 21, 715-737. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20026
|
[11]
|
Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (2013). Principles of Neural Science. New York: McGraw-Hill.
|
[12]
|
Nadel, L., & Jacobs, W. J. (1998). Traumatic Memory Is Special. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 154-157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10836842
|
[13]
|
Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Ohman, A. (2000). Fear and Anxiety: Animal Models and Human Cognitive Psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00343-8
|
[14]
|
Lazar, J. N. (2012). Affect and Attention. Saarbrücken: LAMBERT Academic Publishing.
|
[15]
|
Lazar, J., Kaplan, O., Sternberg, T., & Lubow, R. E. (2012). Positive and Negative Affect Produce Opposing Task-Irrelevant Stimulus Preexposure Effects. Emotion, 12, 591-604. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024867
|
[16]
|
Shachar, K. (2005). The Link between Self-Control, Social Support and Subjective Wellbeing of Teachers in Miftans. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Ramat Aviv: Tel-Aviv University.
|
[17]
|
Schimmack, U. (2005). Response Latencies of Pleasure and Displeasure Ratings: Further Evidence for Mixed Feelings. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 671-691. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930541000020
|
[18]
|
Stone, A. A. (1981). The Association between Perceptions of Daily Experiences and Self- and Spouse-Rated Mood. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 510-522. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(81)90047-7
|
[19]
|
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
|
[20]
|
Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a Consensual Structure of Mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219-235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219
|