[1]
|
Abrams, M., & Reber, A. S. (1988). Implicit Learning: Robustness in the Face of Psychiatric Disorders. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 425-439.
|
[2]
|
Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. (1984). On the Relationship between Task Performance and Associated Verbalisable Knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 209-231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640748408402156
|
[3]
|
Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Issues. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.
|
[4]
|
Broadbent, D. (1977). Levels, Hierarchies and the Locus of Control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 181-201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640747708400596
|
[5]
|
Carr, T. H., & Curran, T. (1994). Cognitive Factors in Learning about Structured Sequences: Applications to Syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 205-230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100012882
|
[6]
|
Chen, G., Tang, Z., Yang, & Dienes (2011). Unconscious Structural Knowledge of Form-meaning Connections. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1751-1760. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.003
|
[7]
|
Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360-365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00168
|
[8]
|
Cleeremans, A. (1997). Principles for Implicit Learning. In D. Berry (Ed.), How Implicit Is Implicit Learning? (pp. 196-234). Oxford: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0008
|
[9]
|
Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., & Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit Learning: News from the Front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406-416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01232-7
|
[10]
|
Cleeremans, A., & Dienes, Z. (2008). Computational Models of Implicit Learning. In R. Sun (Ed.), Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology (pp. 396-421). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816772.018
|
[11]
|
Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the Structure of Event Sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235-253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.235
|
[12]
|
Clegg, B. A., DiGirolamo, G. J., & Keele, S. W. (1998). Sequence Learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 275-281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01202-9
|
[13]
|
Dienes, Z. (1992). Connectionist and Memory-Array Models of Artificial Grammar Learning. Cognitive Science, 16, 41-79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1601_2
|
[14]
|
Dienes, Z. (2008). Subjective Measures of Unconscious Knowledge. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 49-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68005-4
|
[15]
|
Dienes, Z., & Altmann, G. (1997). Transfer of Implicit Knowledge across Domains? How Implicit and How Abstract? In D. Berry (Ed.), How Implicit Is Implicit Learning? (pp. 107-123). Oxford: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523512.003.0005
|
[16]
|
Dienes, Z., Altmann, G. T. M., & Gao, S. J. (1999). Mapping across Domains without Feedback: A Neural Network Model of Transfer of Implicit Knowledge. Cognitive Science, 23, 53-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2301_3
|
[17]
|
Dienes, Z., & Berry, D. C. (1997). Implicit Learning: Below the Subjective Threshold. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 3-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210769
|
[18]
|
Dienes, Z., Broadbent, D. E., & Berry, D. C. (1991). Implicit and Explicit Knowledge Bases in Artificial Grammar Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 17, 875-882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.875
|
[19]
|
Dienes, Z., & Scott, R. (2005). Measuring Unconscious Knowledge: Distinguishing Structural Knowledge and Judgment Knowledge. Psychological Research, 69, 338-351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0208-3
|
[20]
|
Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding Structure in Time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179-211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1402_1
|
[21]
|
Estes, W. K. (1957). Toward a Statistical Theory of Learning. Psychological Review, 57, 94-107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0058559
|
[22]
|
Frensch, P. A., & Rünger, D. (2003). Implicit Learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 13-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01213
|
[23]
|
Fu, Q., Dienes, Z., & Fu, X. (2010). Can Unconscious Knowledge Allow Control in Sequence Learning? Consciousness & Cognition, 19, 462-475. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.10.001
|
[24]
|
Fu, Q., Fu, X., & Dienes, Z. (2008). Implicit Sequence Learning and Conscious Awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 185-202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.007
|
[25]
|
Hintzmann, D. (1986). “Schema Abstraction” in a Multiple-Trace Memory Model. Psychological Review, 93, 411-428. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411
|
[26]
|
Holender, D. (1986). Semantic Activation without Conscious Identification in Dichotic Listening, Parafoveal Vision, and Visual Masking: A Survey and Appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00021269
|
[27]
|
James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/11059-000
|
[28]
|
Jiménez, L., Méndez, C., & Cleeremans, A. (1996). Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Implicit Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 948-969. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.948
|
[29]
|
Kinder, A., & Shanks, D. R. (2001). Amnesia and the Declarative/Nondeclarative Distinction: A Recurrent Network Model of Classification, Recognition, and Repetition Priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 648-669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892901750363217
|
[30]
|
Kinder, A., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Neuropsychological Dissociations between Priming and Recognition: A Single-System Connectionist Account. Psychological Review, 110, 728-744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.728
|
[31]
|
Leung, J., & Williams, J. (2006). Implicit Learning of Form-Meaning Connections. In R. Sun, & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 465-470). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
|
[32]
|
Merikle, P. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 224-233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.224
|
[33]
|
Merikle, P. M., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. D. (2001). Perception without Awareness: Perspectives from Cognitive Psychology. Cognition, 79, 115-134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00126-8
|
[34]
|
Millward, R. B., & Reber, A. S. (1968). Event-Recall in Probability Learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 980-989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(68)80056-8
|
[35]
|
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional Requirement of Learning: Evidence from Performance Measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
|
[36]
|
O’Brien, G., & Opie, J. (1999). A Connectionist Theory of Phenomenal Experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 127-196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X9900179X
|
[37]
|
Payne, J. W. (1994). Thinking Aloud: Insights into Information Processing. Psychological Science, 5, 241-248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00620.x
|
[38]
|
Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A. (1998). PARSER: A Model for Word Segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 246-263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2576
|
[39]
|
Pike, R. (1973). Response Latency Models for Signal Detection. Psychological Review, 80, 53-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0033871
|
[40]
|
Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention in Cognitive Neuroscience: An Overview. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 615-624). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[41]
|
Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of Artificial Grammar Learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227-244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.227
|
[42]
|
Ratcliff, R., & Murdock, B. B. (1976). Retrieval Processes in Recognition Memory. Psychological Review, 83, 190-214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.190
|
[43]
|
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit Learning of artificial Grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855-863. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80149-X
|
[44]
|
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219-235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219
|
[45]
|
Rebuschat, P. (2008). Implicit Learning of Natural Language Syntax. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Cambridge: University of Cambridge.
|
[46]
|
Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. (2009). Implicit Learning of Word Order. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
|
[47]
|
Rieckmann, A & Backman, L. (2009). Implicit Learning in Aging: Extant Patterns and New Directions. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 490-503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9117-y
|
[48]
|
Reingold, E. M., & Merikle, P. M. (1990). On the Interrelatedness of Theory and Measurement in the Study of Unconscious Processes. Mind and Language, 5, 9-28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1990.tb00150.x
|
[49]
|
Rohmeier, M., & Cross, I. (2010). Narmour’s Principles Affect Implicit Learning of Melody. In Demorest et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC 2010), Seattle, 23-27 August 2010.
|
[50]
|
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction (pp. 3-32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.003
|
[51]
|
Searle, J. R. (1992). The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[52]
|
Shanks, D. R. (1997). Distributed Representations and Implicit Knowledge: A Brief Introduction. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, Concepts and Categories (pp. 197-214). Hove: Psychology Press.
|
[53]
|
Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit Learning. In K. Lamberts & R. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook of Cognition (pp. 202-220). London: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781848608177.n8
|
[54]
|
Shanks, D. R., & Perruchet, P. (2002). Dissociation between Priming and Recognition in the Expression of Sequential Knowledge. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 362-367. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196294
|
[55]
|
Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of Dissociable Human Learning Systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367-447. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00035032
|
[56]
|
Shanks, D. R., Wilkinson, L., & Channon, S. (2003). Relationship between Priming and Recognition in Deterministic and Probabilistic Sequence Learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 248-261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.248
|
[57]
|
Stadler, M. A., & Roediger III, H. L. (1998). The Question of Awareness in Research on Implicit Learning. In M. A. Stadler & P. A. Frensch (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Learning (pp. 105-132). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
|
[58]
|
Sun, R. (2002). Duality of the Mind. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
|
[59]
|
Wan, L. L., Dienes, Z., & Fu, X. L. (2008). Intentional Control Based on Familiarity in Artificial Grammar Learning. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 1209-1218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.007
|
[60]
|
Williams, J. N. (2004). Implicit Learning of Form-Meaning Connections. In B. VanPatten, J. Williams, S. Rott, & M. Overstreet (Eds.), Form-Meaning Connections in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 203-218). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
|
[61]
|
Williams, J. N. (2005). Learning without Awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 269-304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0272263105050138
|
[62]
|
Williams, J. N. (2009). Implicit Learning. In W. C. Ritchie, & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), New Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 319-353). Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Ltd.
|