Gender and Overconfidence: Effects of Context, Gendered Stereotypes, and Peer Group

Abstract

Research on Swedish school children has found that boys tend to be overconfident about their grades in mathematics, while girls tend to be underconfident. We find similar results for El Salvadorian children. Mathematics is considered a masculine task and we show that these findings do not carry over to a gender neutral task (social science), where both sexes tend to be overconfident. We find that girls in a single-sex school are more underconfident in their mathematics abilities than girls in a co-ed school, which may suggest that gender stereotypes become reinforced in single-sex environments.

Share and Cite:

Jakobsson, N. , Levin, M. & Kotsadam, A. (2013). Gender and Overconfidence: Effects of Context, Gendered Stereotypes, and Peer Group. Advances in Applied Sociology, 3, 137-141. doi: 10.4236/aasoci.2013.32018.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Andersen, S., Bulte, E., Gneezy, U., & List, J. (2008). Do women supply more public goods than men? Preliminary experimental evidence from matrilineal and patriarchal societies. American Economic Re view, 98, 376-381. doi:10.1257/aer.98.2.376
[2] Beckmann, D., & Menkhoff, L. (2008). Will women be women? Analyzing the gender difference among financial experts. Kyklos, 61, 364-384. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6435.2008.00406.x
[3] Beyer, S. (1990). Gender differences in the accuracy of self-evaluations of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 960-970. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.960
[4] Beyer, S., & Bowden, E. (1997). Gender differences in self-perceptions: Convergent evidence from three measures of accuracy and bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 157-172. doi:10.1177/0146167297232005
[5] Booth, A. L., & Nolen, P. (2012a). Gender differences in risk behaviour: Does nurture matter? Economic Journal, 122, F56-F78. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2011.02480.x
[6] Booth, A. L., & Nolen, P. (2012b). Choosing to compete: How different are girls and boys? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81, 542-555. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2011.07.018
[7] Campbell, A. (2002). A mind of her own: The evolutionary psychology of women. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198504986.001.0001
[8] Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47, 448-474. doi:10.1257/jel.47.2.448
[9] Dahlbom, L., Jakobsson, A., Jakobsson, N., & Kotsadam, A. (2010). Gender and overconfidence: Are girls really overconfident? Applied Economics Letters, 18, 325-327. doi:10.1080/13504851003670668
[10] Deaves, R., Lüders, E., & Schroder, M. (2010). The dynamics of over confidence: Evidence from stock market forecasters. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 17, 402-412. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2010.05.001
[11] Dreber, A., von Essen, E., & Ranehill, E. (2011). Outrunning the gender gap: Boys and girls compete equally. Experimental Economics, 14, 567-582. doi:10.1007/s10683-011-9282-8
[12] Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (2008). Differences in the economic decisions of men and women: Experimental evidence. In C. Plott, & V. Smith (Eds.), Handbook of experimental results (Vol. 1, Chapter 57, pp. 509-519). New York: Elsevier. doi:10.1016/S1574-0722(07)00057-1
[13] Estes, R., & Jinos, H. (1988). The gender gap on Wall Street: An empirical analysis of confidence in investment decision making. Journal of Psychology, 122, 577-590. doi:10.1080/00223980.1988.9915532
[14] Fellner, G., & Maciejovsky, B. (2007). Risk attitude and market behavior: Evidence from experimental asset markets. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 338-350. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.01.006
[15] Gneezy, U., Niederle, M., & Rustichini, A. (2003). Performance in competitive environments: Gender differences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1049-1074. doi:10.1162/00335530360698496
[16] Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2004). Gender and competition at a young age. American Economic Review, 94, 377-381. doi:10.1257/0002828041301821
[17] Gneezy, U., Leonard, K., & List, J. (2006). Gender differences in competition: Evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society. Econometrica, 77, 1637-1664. doi:10.1126/science.1154094
[18] Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320, 1164-1165. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2010.05.003
[19] Günther, C., Ekinci, A., Schwieren, C., & Strobel, M. (2010). Women can’t jump?—An experiment on competitive attitudes and stereotype threat. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 75, 395 401.
[20] Hausmann, R., Tyson, L. D., & Zahidi, S. (2012). The global gender gap report 2012. http://www3.weforum.org/docs/WEF_GenderGap_Report_2012.pdf
[21] Hoxby, C. (2000). Peer effects in the classroom: Learning from gender and race variation. NBER Working Paper 7867.
[22] Jakobsson, N., & Kotsadam, A. (2010). Do attitudes toward gender equality really differ between Norway and Sweden? Journal of European Social Policy, 20, 142-159. doi:10.1177/0958928709358790
[23] Johansson-Stenman, O., & Nordblom, K. (2010). Are men really more overconfident than women? A natural field Experiment on exam behavior. Working Papers in Economics No. 461, University of Gothenburg.
[24] Koellinger, P., Minniti, M., & Schade, C. (2007). “I think I can, I think I can”: Overconfidence and entrepreneurial behaviour. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 502-527. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2006.11.002
[25] Lundberg, M., Fox, P. & Punchchar, J. (1994). Highly confident but wrong: Gender differences and similarities in confidence judgements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 114-121. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.114
[26] Meier-Pesti, K., & Penz, E. (2008). Sex or gender? Expanding the sex-based view by introducing masculinity and femininity as predictors of financial risk taking. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 180-196. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.05.002
[27] Nekby, L., Skogman Thoursie, P., & Vahtrik, L. (2008). Gender and self-selection into a competitive environment: Are women more overconfident than men? Economics Letters, 100, 405-407. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2008.03.005
[28] Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 1067-1101. doi:10.1162/qjec.122.3.1067
[29] Ortmann, A., & Tichy, L. (1999). Gender differences in the laboratory: Evidence from prisoner’s dilemma games. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 39, 327-339. doi:10.1016/S0167-2681(99)00038-4
[30] Palomino, F., & Peyrache, E. A. (2010), Psychological bias and gender agwe gap. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 76, 563-573. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2010.07.004
[31] Soll, J,. & Klayman, J. (2004). Overconfidence in interval estimates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 299-314. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.299
[32] Svensson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta Psychologica, 47, 143-148. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(81)90005-6
[33] Tidemann, J. (2000). Parents’ gender stereotypes and teachers’ beliefs as predictors of children’s concept of their mathematical ability in elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 144-151. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.144

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.