[1]
|
Anderson, J. R. (1991). Is human cognition adaptive? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 471-517. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00070801
|
[2]
|
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R., & Lycett, J. (2002). Human evolutionary psychology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
|
[3]
|
Bayen, U. J., Murnane, K., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). Source discrimination, item detection, and multinomial models of source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 197-215. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.197
|
[4]
|
Br?der, A., & Meiser, T. (2007). Measuring source memory. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215, 52-60.
|
[5]
|
Buss, D. M. (1992). Mate preference mechanisms: Consequences for partner choice and intrasexual competition. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Hrsg.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. (pp. 249-266). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
|
[6]
|
Buss, D. M. (2003). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
|
[7]
|
Butler, A. C., Kang, S. H. K., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Congruity effects between materials and processing tasks in the survival processing paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1477-1486. doi:10.1037/a0017024
|
[8]
|
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the social sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
|
[9]
|
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187-276. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(89)90023-1
|
[10]
|
De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). Impression formation and cooperative behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 305-328.
doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199903/05)29:2/3<305::AID-EJSP929>3.0.CO;2-R
|
[11]
|
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408-423. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.6.408
|
[12]
|
Flexser, A. J., & Tulving, E. (1978). Retrieval independence in recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 85, 153-171.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.85.3.153
|
[13]
|
Godden, D., & Baddeley, A. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325-331. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
|
[14]
|
Howe, M. L., & Derbish, M. H. (2010). On the susceptibility of adaptive memory to false memory illusions. Cognition, 115, 252-267.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.016
|
[15]
|
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3-28.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3
|
[16]
|
Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Cohen, S. M. (2008). The mnemonic advantage of processing fitness-relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1151-1156. doi:10.3758/MC.36.6.1151
|
[17]
|
Kroneisen, M. (2010). On the nature of the survival processing effect. Unpublished Dissertation, Mannheim: University of Mannheim.
|
[18]
|
Kroneisen, M. & Erdfelder, E. (2011). On the plasticity of the survival processing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37, 1553-1562. doi:10.1037/a0024493
|
[19]
|
Meiser, T., & Br?der, A. (2002). Memory for multidimensional source information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 116-137. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.116
|
[20]
|
Murnane, K., & Bayen, U. J. (1996). An evaluation of empirical measures of source identification. Memory & Cognition, 24, 417-428.
doi:10.3758/BF03200931
|
[21]
|
Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. (2008). Adaptive memory: Remembering with a stone-age brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 239-243. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00582.x
|
[22]
|
Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2011). Congruity effects in the survival processing paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 539-549.
doi:10.1037/a0021960
|
[23]
|
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science, 19, 176-180. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02064.x
|
[24]
|
Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 263-273.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.263
|
[25]
|
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N., Smith, M. A., Grimaldi, P. J., & Bauernschmidt, A. (2010). Adaptive memory: Does survival processing enhance nenory for source? 51st Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St Louis, 18-21 November 2010.
|
[26]
|
Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2010). Adaptive memory: Survival processing increases both true and false memory in adults and children. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1010-1016. doi:10.1037/a0019402
|
[27]
|
Otgaar, H., Smeets, T., & Van Bergen, S. (2010). Picturing survival memories: Enhanced memory after fitness-relevant processing occurs for verbal and visual stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 38, 23-28.
doi:10.3758/MC.38.1.23
|
[28]
|
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 45, 255-287.
|
[29]
|
Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positivenegative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology. (Vol. 1, pp. 33-60). Chichester: Wiley.
|
[30]
|
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton & Co.
|
[31]
|
Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1987). A neuropsychological study of fact memory and source amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 464-473.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.464
|
[32]
|
Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34-50.
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34
|
[33]
|
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 6, 174-215. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.174
|
[34]
|
Soderstrom, N. C., & McCabe, D. P. (2011). Are survival processing memory advantages based on ancestral priorities? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 564-569. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0060-6
|
[35]
|
Spencer, W. D., & Raz, N. (1995). Differential effects of aging on memory for content and context: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 10, 527-539. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.527
|
[36]
|
Weinstein, Y., Bugg, J. M., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Can the survival recall advantage be explained by basic memory process? Memory & Cognition, 36, 913-919. doi:10.3758/MC.36.5.913
|
[37]
|
Wilkinson, L. & The Task Force on Statistical Inference (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594-604.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594
|