[1]
|
Corcoran, D. W. J., & Rouse, R. O. (1970). An aspect of perceptual organization involved in reading typed and handwritten words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 526-530.
doi:10.1080/14640747008401930
|
[2]
|
De Zuniga, C. M., Humphreys, G. W., & Evett, L. J. (1991). Additive and interactive effects of repetition, degradation, and word frequency in the reading of handwriting. In D. Besner, & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 10-33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
|
[3]
|
Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. New York: Psychological Press.
|
[4]
|
Job, R., Rumiati, R., & Lotto, L. (1992). The picture superiority effect in categorization: Visual or semantic? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 1019-1028.
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.1019
|
[5]
|
Job, R., & Tenconi, E. L. (1002). Naming pictures at no cost: Asymmetries in picture and word conditional naming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 790-794. doi:10.3758/BF03196336
|
[6]
|
Lloyd-Jones, T. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1997). Categorizing chairs and naming pears: Category differences in object processing as a function of task and priming. Memory & Cognition, 25, 606-624.
doi:10.3758/BF03211303
|
[7]
|
McBride, D. M., & Dosher, B. A. (2002). A comparison of conscious and automatic memory processes for picture and word stimuli: A process dissociation analysis. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 423-460. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00007-7
|
[8]
|
McCann, R. S., & Besner, D. (1987). Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 13, 14-24.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.14
|
[9]
|
Miller, P. (2005). Changes in the processing of letters, written words, and pseudo-homophones: A comparison of fifth graders and university students. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 407-434.
doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.4.407-434
|
[10]
|
Nelson, D. L. (1979). Remembering pictures and words: Appearance, significance, and name. In L. S. Cermak, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory (pp. 45-75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
|
[11]
|
Nelson, D. L., Redd, V. S., & Walling, J. R. (1976). Pictorial superiority effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 523-528. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.2.5.523
|
[12]
|
Paivio, A. (1975). Coding distinctions and repetition effects in memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 179-214). New York: Academic Press.
|
[13]
|
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
|
[14]
|
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255-287.
doi:10.1037/h0084295
|
[15]
|
Paivio, A. (1995). Imagery and memory. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 977-986). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[16]
|
Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1973). Picture superiority in free recall: Imagery or dual coding? Cognitive Psychology, 5, 176-206.
doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90032-7
|
[17]
|
Posner, M. I., & Mitchell, R. R. (1967). Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review, 74, 392-409.
doi:10.1037/h0024913
|
[18]
|
Potter, M., Kroll, J., Yachzel, B., Carpenter, E., & Sherman, J. (1986). Picture in sentences: Understanding without words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 281-294.
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.115.3.281
|
[19]
|
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
|
[20]
|
Shimron, J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 52-67.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.52
|
[21]
|
Weldon, M. S., & Coyote, K. C. (1996). Failure to find the picture superiority effect in implicit conceptual memory tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 670-686. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.670
|
[22]
|
Weldon, M. S., & Roediger, H. L. (1987). Altering retrieval demands reverses the picture superiority effect. Memory & Cognition, 21, 519-528. doi:10.3758/BF03197183
|
[23]
|
Wippich, W., Melzer, A., & Mecklenbrauker, S. (1998). Picture or word superiority effects in implicit memory: Levels of processing, attention, and retrieval constraints. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 57, 33-46.
|