The Separate Development of Children’s Listener and Speaker Behavior and the Intercept as Behavioral Metamorphosis

Abstract

The study of verbal behavior focuses on communicative functions of the speakers/producers as they affect the behavior of listeners/observers. Effects on the listener reinforce the speaker and the listener/observer benefits (i.e., is reinforced) from the behavior of the speaker/producer. The interlocking of, and exchange of, the speaker and listener behavior between individuals and within one’s own skin constitute bidirectional operants. These bidirectional operants are instances of social interactions and measures of social behavior. Evidence suggests that the act of listening, among other observing responses, is initially developmentally independent from speaker behavior. How they become joined parallels the biological phenomenon of metamorphosis. The succession of changes has been empirically identified as a succession of verbal behavior development cusps, which are described in their sequence biologically as a manifestation of functional metamorphosis. The onset of a cusp constitutes first instances of behavior and accompanying stimulus control that allows infants and children to contact parts of the environment for the first time resulting in their learning things impossible to learn before or learning faster. Cusps for the intercept of the speaker and listener lead to bidirectional operants and provide explanations for how children incidentally learn the names of things, become increasingly social, and make subsequent complex behavior possible. Many of the cusps identified in our research resulted from the missing behavior and stimulus control of children with autism. Once cusps were established, these children learned things they could not learn before, learned faster, and learned by contacting parts of the social environment they could not contact before. These findings led to a theory of verbal behavior development that point to the selection of bidirectional operants as behavioral phenotypes during functional metamorphosis, which has enhanced the survival of Homo sapiens through emergent symbolic skills for more effective collaboration between two or more individuals.

Share and Cite:

Greer, R. , Pohl, P. , Du, L. and Moschella, J. (2017) The Separate Development of Children’s Listener and Speaker Behavior and the Intercept as Behavioral Metamorphosis. Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, 7, 674-704. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2017.713045.

1. Introduction

When verbal behavioral developmental repertoires and the pre-verbal foundational repertoires are demonstrated for the first time they are described as verbal behavior developmental cusps (e.g., first incidences of mands or requests, tacts or declaratives, or the incidental learning of the names of things or bidirectional naming (BiN). The cusps that are present, or not present, in children’s repertoires determine the degree to which children: 1) communicate, 2) contact social contingencies, 3) learn from incidental experiences, and 4) do, or do not, learn from different types of instruction. The behaviors of communicating include: 1) language, or the lexicon (e.g., a dictionary and grammar of a particular language), and 2) the range of behaviors that constitute how individuals affect (i.e., communicate to) and are affected (i.e., communicated with) by each other’s behavior. We use this meaning of the term verbal consistent with Skinner’s [1] [2] usage, as well as the expansion of the meaning of verbal resulting from decades of research in verbal behavior development and relational stimulus control [3] [4] . Verbal behavior is communicative behavior that embraces the lexicon of language as well as the vocal and non-vocal behaviors of communication.

Verbal behavior develops in children’s lifetime as a result of learning new social reinforcers for observing (e.g., listening and looking) and producing responses (e.g., speaking as a result of social stimulus control). These newly learned reinforcers, in turn, lead to the emergence of learned motivating conditions and new antecedent stimulus control, including the frames of relational stimulus control [4] [5] . This new stimulus control was not possible before the onset of certain verbal developmental cusps. VBDT argues that these experiences and the wherewithal to contact them constitute the environmental sources for the kinds of neurological development documented by Kuhl and colleagues [6] [7] [8] . Thus, cusps are part of contextual control involved or make that contextual control possible. For example, the particular function of a word, saying the word fox as a type of mand/request (i.e., “Look out!”) or tact/declarative (“Oh yes, I see it”), depends on the context. But what is context? We argue that it is, in part, the particular learned reinforcers for using that word socially. The use, or intended function, depends on a particular reinforcement function and, in turn, the motivating conditions associated with a particular reinforcer. In behavior analysis, each type of reinforcement is accompanied with a motivating condition. For example, swallowing water reinforces drinking and a related motivating conditions might be the consumption of salt. A newly learned reinforcer (i.e., the sound of mother’s voice learned as a reinforcer for observing in utero) also results in new motivating conditions (i.e., deprivation of hearing mother’s voice enhances reinforcement for observing). Together, the newly learned motivating condition and the newly learned reinforcer make the relevant stimulus discriminatively salient (i.e., stimuli signaling the appearance of mother’s voice such as the sound of footsteps). Different contexts consist of different learned social reinforcement or punishment contingencies that act to select the particular discriminative stimulus out of many. A history that leads to multiple learned reinforcers for various behaviors establishes this contextual control as we describe in the tables that follow.

Research findings from other disciplinary approaches to development have demonstrated the importance of ontogeny (i.e., experience) including comparative psychology [9] , cognitive developmental psychology [10] , and developmental neuroscience [6] . The role of experience has also been identified in behavioral analysis of complex human behavior and is its research focus [4] [11] . Behavioral analyses of verbal behavior development contribute by identifying how children come to contact parts of the environment that then provide the experiential stimuli for their contacting still other experiences. In this context, and with a view towards the far-reaching behavioral changes children regularly show during language development, we began asking ourselves whether it would be productive―both from a theoretical and applied perspective-to view some of these conspicuous transformations in behavioral functionality as exemplars of behavioral metamorphosis.

We remembered that Skinner [12] , Catania [13] [14] [15] [16] , and Donahoe [17] provided strong arguments for the functional similarities between natural selection in phylogeny and selection by reinforcement during ontogeny, and that Donahoe [18] had reviewed state-of-the-art evolutionary biology from the perspective of current developments in the experimental analysis of behavior. But a comprehensive search of the literature on metamorphosis and behavior only turned up one brief discussion of metamorphosis and adaptation in the behavior of developing mammals [19] .

The lack of findings in the biology research literature on metamorphosis eventually led us to Charles Darwin and one of his far-sighted intuitions. Commenting on the birth of Haeckel’s child, Darwin noted in a letter to him in 1865, “You will be astonished to find how the whole mental disposition of your child changes with advancing years. A young child, and the same when nearly grown, sometimes differ almost as much as do a caterpillar and butterfly” [20] . The body of research on verbal behavior development represented in what follows underlines Darwin’s intuition in that it suggests that differences in children’s being able to contact critical parts of their environment before and after the emergence of specific developmental cusps are similar to the effects of structural metamorphosis on organisms but on a functional or behavioral level.

These findings in verbal behavior development within both basic and applied science are implicitly translational. Figure 1 displays the verbal behavior

Figure 1. Verbal developmental milestones as behavioral metamorphosis in humans and structural metamorphosis in the life cycle of the butterfly.

development of individuals in parallel to the metamorphosis of a butterfly. Readers can scan the QR code below for a dynamic display of this figure1.

2. Onset of Verbal Developmental Cusps as Metamorphosis

Education and development are inextricably intertwined. From the behavior analytic perspective, it is experience, not the passage of time alone, that leads to development in an individual [21] [22] [23] [24] . There is a long history of behavior analyses of development that includes pioneers such as Jacob Gerwirtz, Sidney Bijou, Donald Baer, Barbara Etzel, Betty Hartman, and Todd Risley, to name a few [25] . Verbal Behavior Developmental Theory (VBDT) and research [3] [26] [27] [28] builds on that history and extends it to behavioral analysis of development and verbal behavior. The analysis focuses on how verbal behavior develops from experience, including how children come to contact experiences, and how that development determines how and what a child can be taught directly or learn incidentally.

Children who lack the presence of cusps and capabilities are first identified by measures of their rate of learning in curricular areas (i.e., instructional trials to mastery, learning from instruction and demonstration. Detailed algorithms test for the presence or absence of a cusp or a cusp that is also a new learning capability (e.g., child demonstrates they learn from vicarious contact with instruction or exposure and no instruction) [26] [27] . Children may lack a cusp, either because they lack environmental experiences that form the cusp (i.e., environmental impoverishment), or as a result of native disabilities. Children’s instructional and experiential histories may or may not contain the key experiences that establish verbal developmental (VBD) cusps. Children who are exposed to a rich verbal community are more likely to acquire VBD cusps, and by contrast, children who come from low-language interaction homes may not acquire them [25] . Many children diagnosed with autism are missing one or more cusps. The induction of cusps and capabilities can radically change a child such that they can contact experiences that, in turn, allow the child to learn things they could not before, learn faster, or learn in new ways [24] [29] . Teachers, in turn, need to change the way they teach to match the countless new opportunities to learn that the child can now contact. For example, providing instruction and examples prior to providing response opportunities leads to faster outcomes if a child demonstrates the cusp of bidirectional BiN [30] ; however, if a child lacks that cusp, they learn little, if anything, from response-antecedent instruction and must learn mostly from direct consequences [31] . Moreover, certain cusps make it possible for learning multiple responses to the same stimulus from learning one [32] , as is the case when transformation of stimulus function across saying and writing is present and the child demonstrates incidental language learning (BiN) [33] . In the latter case, simply hearing a word spoken for an object can result in the child learning the word-object relation (i.e., “meaning” of the word), learning how to spell the word by speaking the letter names, and writing the word, all without direct instruction. In addition, any respondent events (i.e., emotions) occurring as the word is spoken relative to the “referent” may become part of the stimulus control of any of the learning outcomes. Figure 2 shows how these cusps are related to each stage of verbal development and are thus analogous to the metamorphosis of a butterfly.

Verbal behavior developmental research seeks to identify specific sequences of experiences (the basic science) and developmental interventions (the applied science) that lead to the establishment of these cusps. The current evidence suggests that many of these cusps result from establishing new or learned reinforcers that simultaneously establish new motivational conditions [23] [34] [35] [36] . These protocols provide the key experiences that result in conditioning

Figure 2. Four verbal behavior developmental metamorphosis stages.

new reinforcers and consequently lead to verbal development. Examples include a protocol to condition adults’ faces and voices for observing adults, resulting in faster rates of learning [37] , and a conditioning protocol to establish print as reinforcement for observing, resulting in generalized visual match to sample [38] [39] [40] . These latter cusps are pre-verbal foundational cusps as shown in Table 1, with the research-based protocols to induce them. Pre-verbal foundational cusps typically occur in utero and infancy but are found missing in children with severe developmental delays.

The developmental trajectory involves a history of a child contacting what are initially independent observing (e.g., looking and listening) and producing responses (speaking) [1] [45] that become coordinated or joined as a result of contact with contingencies. An example on the observing side is the progression from: 1) observing mom’s face after birth as a result of vocal sounds having been conditioned in utero and then paired with the mom’s face after birth, to 2) listening to phonemes. The production responses of babbling contact relations with the mother’s voice and emulation of mom’s sounds (i.e., parroting or

Table 1. Verbal behavior developmental milestones: Pre-verbal foundational cusps.

canonical babbling). Previously learned reinforcement value for mom’s voice leads to the learning of correspondence between mom’s and baby’s speech as reinforcement that is the source parroting/canonical babbling [46] . Parroting is pre-verbal speech (i.e., not affected by the behavior of another listener) but sets the stage for the child emitting spoken sounds that, in turn, produces a magical result, a verbal effect on another. When the parroted word enters a mand or tact function, this is a major step towards becoming verbal. The speaker responses then expand as the child contacts more echoic to mand or echoic to tact experiences. The development of the functional verbal repertoires such as mands and tacts allows the child to respond to natural establishing operations and recruit social attention from adults and peers more readily. However, until the individual rotates the listener and speaker functions as a dialogue, as in self-talk alternation of listener and speaker, as in fantasy play, the child is not fully verbal. Table 2 lists the listener and speaker cusps, as they exist independently, before they are joined and the child is fully verbal.

One becomes truly verbal when the speaker and listener are joined such that the child talks to herself. The evidence for this includes research on see and do correspondence [74] , self-talk in fantasy play [68] , and BiN (bidirectional operants) [52] [75] [76] , as listed in Table 3. Once this relation is present, contact with experiences and instruction lead to the benefits of following algorithms as listener/reader and producing algorithms as speaker/writer. From a radical behavioral perspective, this intersection of listener and speaker constitutes verbal thinking [1] [2] [3] . One could even argue this is the onset of consciousness since it would appear that non-verbal consciousness is not viable even as a cognitive psychological construct. We maintain that the intersect of speaker and listener during verbal development is a veritable manifestation of behavioral metamorphosis. The comparative biology behind this contention is discussed together with its theoretical implications in the coda following our further delineation of the empirical milestones of verbal development.

The three cusps that constitute new learning capabilities are: Generalized imitation [77] [83] , observational learning [91] [92] [93] [95] [96] [97] and BiN [28] [75] [89] . By capabilities we mean that the child reliably demonstrates ways to learn that they could not do before experience allowed them to do so. The chrysalis forms as the numbers and types of independent speaker and listener responses accrue. With the onset of generalized imitation looking and reproducing what is seen becomes a reinforcer because of the correspondence between seeing and doing. This is likely an extension of what happens when an infant finally can reach for what they are observing after considerable attempts to do so. Observing and producing responses intersect. Once a child imitates, because the correspondence is reinforcement [5] [46] , they can be shown what to do and will attempt to duplicate with few or no prompts. Learning from social contact becomes more prevalent. That is, the observing and producing responses become more frequently reinforced by social reinforcement. A more advanced example of this occurs when children come to learn from observing the consequences received by others [98] . The first time the child demonstrates these learning capabilities the sources of reinforcement shifts from reinforcement delivered from instruction to the learned reinforcement of the behavioral effect. At this point, the child does not need to be reinforced by the instructor; rather, the reinforcement is embedded in the new learned reinforcer that is part of the operant. By extension, the new reinforcers then lead to other incidental pairings that, in turn, result in still other learned reinforcers that select out still other behaviors. The shift in the type of reinforcement not only allows one to be more independent, but also results in the hundreds and thousands of new learning opportunities for the individual. The individual has access to the unlimited possibilities. BiN then makes it possible for the print stimuli to join with the auditory stimuli. And that in turn makes all the symbolic relations (i.e., reading,

Table 2. Verbal behavior developmental milestones: Listener and speaker.

writing, and solving complex relations) possible as extensions of the joining of observing and producing responses. Table 4 lists the advanced verbal developmental cusps and corresponding protocols on joining of listener and speaker responses that enable the child to become fully verbal.

Table 3. Verbal behavior developmental cusps that are new learning capabilities.

Research from Kuhl and her colleagues [6] [7] [8] shed light on associating neural commitment to evaluate language development. It is likely that the interplay between neuroscience and the science of verbal behavior could illuminate on more timely and detailed measures of the metamorphosis upon the acquisition of new developmental cusps and capabilities (i.e., BiN). A close look at the brain activities before and after the establishment of verbal behavior developmental cusps can identify particular experiences that affect brain behavior and behavior outside the skin leading to a more complete understanding of the role of experience in language and social development, as well as validate instrumentation. Transdisciplinary collaboration between neuroscience and verbal behavior development may provide more insights in how to propel the desired metamorphosis process, or to reverse one’s environmental adversity, and/ or native disability.

3. Coda: Bidirectional Operants as Behavioral Metamorphosis

The quality of change in verbal functionality which results from the onset of bidirectional operants as the joining of the speaker and listener needs to be addressed. Our research has led us to believe that the transformation which occurs is comparable to that of metamorphosis in biology. In pursuing this hypothesis, we follow Skinner [12] , Catania [13] [14] [15] [16] , and Donahoe [17] , who as mentioned above, have provided strong arguments for the functional similarities between natural selection in phylogeny and selection by reinforcement during ontogeny. In his review of a collection of papers concerned with major developments in evolutionary biology during the 150 years since the publication of Darwin's On the Origin of Species, Donahoe [18] comes to the conclusion that “our understanding of the details of selection by reinforcement is arguably at least as complete as was Darwin's understanding of natural selection” (p. 256).

What is metamorphosis? From the Greek. “meta” for “change” and “morphe” for “form”, metamorphosis as a biological process is extreme life-stage modularity

Table 4. Verbal behavior developmental milestones: Joining of listener and speaker responses.

and means that a single genome produces at least two highly distinctive morphological and behavioral phenotypes which occupy very different habitats. From a quantitative perspective, nearly all of the species on earth―butterflies, bees, beetles, ants, moths, flies, and wasps, as well as many marine invertebrates, amphibians and fish―undergo dramatic phenotypic remodeling during development. Metamorphosis is thus ubiquitous throughout nature and has evolved repeatedly in the history of multicellular life. A number of comparative biologists convened a good decade ago to discuss the phenomenon of metamorphosis [124] , compared it across various taxa in the animal kingdom, and came to the conclusion that metamorphosis was a component of organismic ontogeny. Of particular interest to the present discussion was the finding that, despite considerable differences in the definition of the term, apparently no distinctive life-cycle transitions comparable to metamorphosis in insects (see caterpillar to butterfly) had been documented in mammals.

In her treatise on Developmental Plasticity and Evolution [125] , considered by some as the most important book about evolution since The Origin of Species, evolutionary developmental biologist and entomologist Mary Jane West-Eber- hard devotes a separate chapter to phenotypic recombination due to learning (pp. 337-352), wherein she focuses on the importance of learning as the origin of novel adaptive phenotypes. West-Eberhard notes that “Most people, including most biologists, probably underestimate the importance of learning in the biology of nonhuman animals” and goes on to say that, “… learning itself has not always received the attention it deserves as a phenomenon of general evolutionary interest” (p. 337). Inspired by West-Eberhard’s approach to behavior and learning, a recent paper on epigenetics and the evolution of instincts suggests that in numerous insects, instincts may evolve from learning, and the paper provides evidence that “… the genome responds dynamically to a range of behaviorally relevant stimuli, often with massive changes in brain-gene expression” [126] (p. 26). Based on her extensive research on social insects and comprehensively documented findings from development, physiology and behavior, West-Eberhard’s [125] synthesis of evolutionary biology focused on phenotypic plasticity has been influential to the argument developed in what follows.

In our research on children with various types of language delays, time and again we have been struck by the profound behavioral transformation which regularly occurs when novel verbal capabilities are induced in these children on the basis of an applied behavior analysis systems approach to teaching [26] [127] combined with Horne and Lowe’s seminal account of BiN in language development [75] . The latter authors noted that “… a child, through learning listener behavior and then echoic responding, learns bidirectional relations between classes of objects or events and his or her own speaker-listener behavior, thus acquiring naming―a higher order behavioral relation” (p. 185), and thus provided a first description of what is now terminologically called bidirectional naming or BiN [76] . Given that such bidirectional responding usually emerges untaught once children reared in a language-rich environment have acquired listener, echoic and tacting repertoires, we believe that the bidirectional verbal operant merits special consideration as a potential candidate for behavioral metamorphosis during language development [128] .

In his “Biographical Sketch of an Infant”, it was Darwin [129] again who made a keen observation in one of his own children, and it serves as an excellent starting point for the introduction of the bidirectional operant as an instance of behavioral metamorphosis from the perspective of evolutionary developmental biology. Darwin wrote:

“At exactly the age of a year, he made the great step of inventing a word for food, namely mum, but what led him to it I did not discover. And now instead of beginning to cry when he was hungry, he used this word in a demonstrative manner or as a verb, implying ‘Give me food’… I was particularly struck with the fact that when asking for food by the word mum he gave to it (I will copy the words written down at the time) ‘a most strongly marked interrogatory sound at the end’. He also gave to ‘Ah,’ which he chiefly used at first when recognizing any person or his own image in a mirror, an exclamatory sound, such as we employ when surprised. I remark in my notes that the use of these intonations seemed to have arisen instinctively, and I regret that more observations were not made on this subject. I record, however, in my notes that at a rather later period, when between 18 and 21 months old, he modulated his voice in refusing peremptorily to do anything by a defiant whine, so as to express ‘That I won't’; and again his humph of assent expressed ‘Yes, to be sure’… The interrogatory sound which my child gave to the word mum when asking for food is especially curious; for if anyone will use a single word or a short sentence in this manner, he will find that the musical pitch of his voice rises considerably at the close. I did not then see that this fact bears on the view which I have elsewhere maintained that before man used articulate language, he uttered notes in a true musical scale as does the anthropoid ape Hylobates” (p. 293).

Darwin [130] had expressed this view six years earlier in his treatise on “The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex”, Vol. 2, as follows:

“Another gibbon, the H. agilis, is highly remarkable, from having the power of emitting a complete and correct octave of musical notes, which we may reasonably suspect serves as a sexual charm; but I shall have to recur to this subject in the next chapter. The vocal organs of the American Mycetes caraya are one-third larger in the male than in the female, and are wonderfully powerful. These monkeys, when the weather is warm, make the forests resound during the morning and evening with their overwhelming voices. The males begin the dreadful concert, in which the females, with their less powerful voices, sometimes join, and which is often continued during many hours. An excellent observer, Rengger, could not perceive that they were excited to begin their concert by any special cause; he thinks that like many birds, they delight in their own music, and try to excel each other. Whether most of the foregoing monkeys have acquired their powerful voices in order to beat their rivals and to charm the females―or whether the vocal organs have been strengthened and enlarged through the inherited effects of long-continued use without any particular good being gained―I will not pretend to say; but the former view, at least in the case of the Hylobates agilis, seems the most probable” (p. 277).

Of particular interest in this context is so-called “duet-splitting” and “singing” in gibbons (Hylobatidae), documented in a field study by Geissmann [131] . Throughout the tropical rainforests of southeast Asia, in many species of gibbons, mated pairs and unmated couples characteristically combine their songs― stereotyped, loud and long sequences of vocalizations―to produce conversational duets, which apparently have the function of advertising territoriality, attracting mates and, once established, maintaining pair and family bonds.

In the Descent of Man, Darwin [130] wrote that the human musical faculty “must be ranked amongst the most mysterious with which he is endowed” and hypothesized an intermediate stage of human evolution characterized by a means of communication that resembles music more closely than language―a type of prosodic protolanguage―as a precursor to both modern language and music. This topic, which has always been puzzling to evolutionary biologists, has recently been investigated by Masataka [132] [133] . In a similar vein, theorizing with a view towards ‘a more comprehensive understanding of language,’ Greer [134] mentions music and dance in addition to verbal behavior and the visual arts as evolutionary outcomes of the ontogenetic selection of separate observing and producing responses and the cultural joining of them (p. 370)―thus setting the stage for the hypothesis proposed here concerning the evolutionary origins of the bidirectional operant as an instance of behavioral metamorphosis in Homo sapiens.

In a recent descriptive study in baboons, Boë and colleagues [135] suggest that Papio Papio produces vocalizations comparable to human vowels. Employing acoustic analyses of vocalizations coupled with an anatomical study of the tongue muscles and dynamic modeling of the acoustic potential of the vocal tract, the data suggest that baboons, in spite of their high larynx, are capable of producing at least five vocalizations with the properties of vowels, and that they can combine them when they communicate with troop conspecifics. The finding suggests that the last common ancestor of Homo sapiens and Papio papio may have possessed the anatomical wherewithal for speech―hinting at a much earlier evolutionary onset of vocal verbal skills than heretofore assumed. In an observational study on the use of affiliative vocalizations by wild baboons, Silk, Seyfarth and Cheney [136] recently found that despite their limited vocal repertoire, Papio ursinus females were skilled at modifying call production in different social contexts and for different audiences (e.g., vocalizing towards mothers with young infants but not towards females without infants). We may assume that the socially selective production of calls in these female baboons presupposes a complementary selective listening behavior in their female counterparts.

With a view towards speech perception from a comparative perspective, valuable support would be given to the prosodic protolanguage view of the evolution of language if, for example, functional ear asymmetries like those typically found in humans were established experimentally in nonhuman primates. In the first study to document ear advantages for acoustic stimuli in a nonhuman primate species, Pohl [137] found highly significant and reproducible ear advantages in four baboons (Papio cynocephalus) for the monaural discrimination of pure tones, three-tone musical chords, synthetically-constructed consonant-vowels (CVs) and vowels in each of the animals with marked individual differences in the direction of ear asymmetry. The ear advantages found in these baboons under monaural conditions resembled those obtained with dichotic presentation in human subjects [138] and thus suggest―especially when considered together with the recent findings [135] on the vowel-like quality of baboon vocalizations mentioned above―that the baboon may provide a valuable nonhuman primate model of functional asymmetry in the central auditory system. In a subsequent study on ear advantages for temporal resolution in the same four baboons, Pohl [139] hypothesized that the temporal resolution which underlies the individual animal’s ear advantage for speech perception, would predict an ear advantage in a temporal resolution task to correlate precisely with an ear advantage for the discrimination of CVs that differ in their temporal features. A gap detection task that required the resolution of brief intervals of no noise (between 7 and 20 msec depending on the animal) in bursts of random noise was employed using the same experimental paradigm as in the previous study to test the hypothesis in all four baboons. The findings clearly confirmed the hypothesis in that the probability of obtaining by chance the same direction of ear advantage in four animals under two very different task conditions was less than .01. The results suggest that a single perceptual mechanism based on fine temporal analysis underlies the CV discrimination and gap detection tasks in both humans and baboons and that this mechanism may be asymmetrically organized within the central auditory system of primates. These findings in baboons have potential implications for the evolution of hemispheric specialization for speech perception in man [140] . We now turn to comparative studies in birds, which throw further light on the argument developed here.

Any attempt to understand behavioral metamorphosis during speech development from a biological perspective is well advised to take note of a few established facts from studies of birdsong development such as auditory feedback in song development [141] , the discovery of hemispheric lateralization of song control [142] and functional similarities of vocal learning in birds and man [143] [144] . In a recent retrospective of Marler’s pioneering work in the field of birdsong development, Soha and Peters [145] list six basic rules, identified through Maler’s extensive experimental and observational research, which underlie the ontogeny of vocal behavior in both humans and white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys). As they are pivotal to the argument under discussion, we summarize these rules here:

1) Exposure to certain sounds (usually the vocalizations of adult male conspecifics) has a crucial impact on song repertoire of adult songbirds.

2) This exposure has the greatest effect during a certain developmental time range, which means that vocal learning proceeds most readily during an early critical period.

3) Vocal learning is guided by species-specific predispositions: young songbirds are predisposed to pay particular attention to the vocalizations of their own species whereas human babies preferentially attend to the speech of their mothers.

4) Both songbirds and babies must hear their own output during vocal development, as auditory feedback guides the matching of this output to what was heard previously.

5) Some vocal learning in songbirds apparently proceeds without external reinforcement. Horne and Lowe [75] suggest this and Longano and Greer [88] report evidence for learned embedded reinforcement as the source of BiN in children.

6) Vocal motor development proceeds in stages: young individuals begin by making vocal sounds that do not resemble those of adults, and even deafened individuals produce the earliest versions of these sounds (subsong).

Kuhl [8] and her associates have pioneered multidisciplinary research on social and speech development in early childhood and compared it with studies of birdsong development. Kuhl [7] notes that “Both babies and birds must… rehearse and refine their communicative repertoires, actively comparing and gradually matching (through auditory feedback) their productions to the sound patterns stored in auditory memory (p. 9645).”

Taken together and combined with the primate data cited above, these findings suggest that a class of operant behavior underwent selection by reinforcement, enabling modular design of a bidirectional behavioral phenotype as a necessary precondition for the evolution of human speech development. In joining the listener and speaker together in the same skin, the bidirectional operant would seem to exemplify the process of complete behavioral metamorphosis. In terms of our visualization from insect ontogeny, if the caterpillar’s habitat is the ground, branches and leaves, and the pre-verbal child’s world is the immediate physical and social environment, then the butterfly’s new habitat is the air and plants and the novel environment of the fully verbal child is the symbolic universe of a verbal community. Whereas the pre-verbal child behaves more like a bright nonhuman primate in its proximate physical and social environment, the child who has acquired BiN or operant bidirectionality becomes a member of a verbal community, which is the equivalent of a novel environment of symbolic relationships. We propose here that the evolutionary prerequisite for that profound behavioral transformation is the ontogenetic selection of separate observing and producing phenotypes and their modular integration into the bidirectional operant through social learning embedded in a human culture. Based on this proposition, we could envisage the gradual approximation of two sub-disciplines of biology―evolutionary developmental biology and developmental verbal behavior analysis―which would lend further support to the idea of a unified model of behavior [125] .

Finally, the homologies assembled here of metamorphosis across taxa may someday help us understand the riddle of what paleoanthropologist Ian Tattersall [146] depicted in his unsettling evolutionary account entitled “Once We were not Alone”, in which behavior and learning play a central role. The story, which we quote extensively because of its significance for the argument developed here, begins by mentioning that we usually take for granted that Homo sapiens is the only hominid on Earth―despite the fact that for at least four million years many hominid species shared the planet. What makes us different?

“Business had continued more or less as usual right through the appearance of modern bone structure, and only later, with the acquisition of fully modern behavior patterns, did H. sapiens become completely intolerant of competition from its nearest―and, evidently, not its dearest. To understand how this change in sensibility occurred, we have to recall certain things about the evolutionary process. First, as in this case, all innovations must necessarily arise within preexisting species―for where else can they do so? And second, many novelties arise as “exaptations”, features acquired in one context before (often long before) being coopted in a different one. For example, hominids possessed essentially modern vocal tracts for hundreds of thousands of years before the behavioral record gives us any reason to believe that they employed the articulate speech that the peculiar form of this tract permits.” (p. 62).

The author then goes on to say:

“We do know, however, that somehow our lineage passed to symbolic thought from some nonsymbolic precursor state. The only plausible possibility is that with the arrival of anatomically modern H. sapiens, existing exaptations were fortuitously linked by some relatively minor genetic innovation to create an unprecedented potential. Yet even in principle this cannot be the full story, because anatomically modern humans behaved archaically for a long time before adopting modern behaviors. That discrepancy may be the result of the late appearance of some key hardwired innovation not reflected in the skeleton, which is all that fossilizes. But this seems unlikely, because it would have necessitated a wholesale Old World-wide replacement of hominid populations in a very short time, something for which there is no evidence. It is much more likely that the modern human capacity was born at―or close to―the origin of H. sapiens, as an ability that lay fallow until it was activated by a cultural stimulus of some kind (italics ours). If sufficiently advantageous, this behavioral novelty could then have spread rapidly by cultural contact among populations that already had the potential to acquire it. No population replacement would have been necessary. It is impossible to be sure what this innovation might have been, but the best current bet is that it was the invention of language. For language is not simply the medium by which we express our ideas and experiences to each other. Rather it is fundamental to the thought process itself. It involves categorizing and naming objects and sensations in the outer and inner worlds and making associations between resulting mental symbols (italics ours). It is, in effect, impossible for us to conceive of thought (as we are familiar with it) in the absence of language, and it is the ability to form mental symbols that is the fount of our creativity, for only once we create such symbols can we recombine them and ask such questions as ‘What if...?’” (p. 62)

Tattersall [146] ends his account on a memorable note: “We do not know exactly how language might have emerged in one local population of H. sapiens, although linguists have speculated widely. But we do know that a creature armed with symbolic skills is a formidable competitor―and not necessarily an entirely rational one, as the rest of the living world, including H. neanderthalensis, has discovered to its cost” (p. 62).

Given a certain plausibility, the message in Tattersall’s narrative is that if verbal development played a central role in the selection and rapid global dissemination of symbolic behavior in Homo sapiens, it will be future innovation in education and learning which comes from the most rapidly developing members of the species, namely children and adolescents, who will help us most to navigate in what the late German sociologist-philosopher Ulrich Beck has aptly called “The Metamorphosis of the World” [147] . In this development, the significance of bidirectional operants cannot be overestimated.

NOTES

1Readers need to download a QR scanner on smartphones or tablets (i.e., QR reader) to view these QR codes.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Skinner, B.F. (1957) Verbal Behavior. Copley Publishing Group and the B.F. Skinner Foundation, Acton.
[2] Skinner, B.F. (1987) Whatever Happened to Psychology as the Science of Behavior? American Psychologist, 42, 780-786.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.42.8.780
[3] Greer, R.D. and Ross, D.E. (2007) Verbal Behavior Analysis: Inducing and Expanding New Verbal Capabilities in Children with Language Delays. Allyn and Bacon, New York.
[4] Hayes, S.C., Barnes-Holmes, D. and Roche, B., Eds. (2001) Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition. Plenum Press, New York.
[5] Greer, R.D. and Du, L. (2015) Identification and Establishment of Reinforcers that Make the Development of Complex Social Language Possible. International Journal of Behavior Analysis and Autism Disorders, 1, 13-34.
https://ijobasd.org/index.php/IJOBAS/article/view/24
[6] Imada, T., Zhang, Y., Cheour, M., Taulu, S., Ahonen, A. and Kuhl, P.K. (2006) Infant Speech Perception Activates Broca’s Area: A Developmental Magnetoencephalography Study. Neuroreport, 17, 957-962.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000223387.51704.89
[7] Kuhl, P. (2003) Human Speech and Birdsong: Communication and the Social Brain. PNAS, 100, 9645-9646.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1733998100
[8] Kuhl, P. (2004) Early Language Acquisition: Cracking the Speech Code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831-843.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1533
[9] Tomasello, T. and Bates, E. (2001) Language Development: Essential Readings. Blackwell Publishing, Malden.
[10] Crystal, D. (2006) How Language Works: How Babies Babble, Words Change Meaning, and Language Lives or Dies. Overlook Press, Woodstock.
[11] Greer, R.D. and Yuan, L. (2008) How Kids Learn to Say the Darnedest Things: The Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction on Novel Verb Usage. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 103-121.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393060
[12] Skinner, B.F. (1996) The Ethics of Helping People. In: Mattaini, M.A. and Thayer, B.A., Eds., Finding Solutions to Social Problems: Behavioral Strategies for Change, American Psychological Association, Washington DC, 61-72.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10217-003
[13] Catania, A.C. (1995) Higher-Order Behavior Classes: Contingencies, Beliefs, and Verbal Behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 191-200.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(95)00033-V
[14] Catania, A.C. (2001) Three Varieties of Selection and Their Implications for the Origins of Language. Language Evolution: Biological, Linguistic and Philosophical Perspectives, 55-71.
[15] Catania, A.C. (2008) Brain and Behavior: Which Way Does the Shaping Go? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 516-517.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08005062
[16] Catania, A.C. (2013) A Natural Science of Behavior. Review of General Psychology, 17, 133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033026
[17] Donahoe, J.W. (1996) On the Relation between Behavior Analysis and Biology. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 71.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392740
[18] Donahoe, J.W. (2012) Reflections on Behavior Analysis and Evolutionary Biology: A Selective Review of Evolution since Darwin—The First 150 Years. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97, 249-260.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2012.97-249
[19] Oppenheim, R.W. (1980) Metamorphosis and Adaptation in the Behavior of Developing Organisms. Developmental Psychology, 13, 353-356.
https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420130402
[20] Darwin, F. (1911) The Life and Letters of Charles Darwin. Appleton Books, London.
[21] Bijou, S.W. (1996) Reflections on Some Early Events Related to Behavior Analysis of Child Development. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 49-60.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392738
[22] Gewirtz, J.L. (1969) Mechanisms of Social Learning: Some Roles of Stimulation and Behavior in Early Human Development. In: Goslin D.A., Ed., Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research, Rand-McNally, Chicago, 57-72.
[23] Gewirtz, J.L. and Baer, D.M. (1958) Deprivation and Satiation of Social Reinforcers as Drive Conditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 57, 165-172.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042880
[24] Novak, G. and Pelaez, M. (2004) Child and Adolescent Development: A Behavioral Systems Approach. Sage, Thousand Oaks.
[25] Hart, B. and Risley, T.R. (1995) Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Paul H Brookes Publishing, Baltimore.
[26] Greer, R.D. (2002) Designing Teaching Strategies: An Applied Behavior Analysis Systems Approach. Academic Press, New York.
[27] Keohane, D.D. and Greer, R.D. (2005) Teachers’ Use of a Verbally Governed Algorithm and Student Learning. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 1, 252.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100749
[28] Greer, R.D. and Speckman, J. (2009) The Integration of Speaker and Listener Responses: A Theory of Verbal Development. The Psychological Record, 59, 449-488.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395674
[29] Rosales-Ruiz, J. and Baer, D.M. (1997) Behavioral Cusps: A Developmental and Pragmatic Concept for Behavior Analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 533-544.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1997.30-533
[30] Hranchuk, K., Greer, R.D. and Longano, J. Instructional Demonstrations Are more Effective than Consequences Alone for Children with Naming. Analysis of Verbal Behavior. (In Press)
[31] Greer, R.D., Corwin, A. and Buttigieg, S. (2011) The Effects of the Verbal Developmental Capability of Naming on How Children Can Be Taught. Acta de Investigacion Psicologia, 1, 23-54.
http://scielo.unam.mx/pdf/aip/v1n1/v1n1a5.pdf
[32] Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y. and Cullinan, V. (2001) Relational Frame Theory and Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 69-84.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392000
[33] Greer, R.D., Yuan, L. and Gautreaux, G. (2005) Novel Dictation and Intraverbal Responses as a Function of a Multiple Exemplar History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99-116.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393012
[34] Michael, J. (1993) Concepts and Principles of Behavior Analysis. Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo.
[35] Michael, J. (1993) Establishing Operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191-206.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392623
[36] Nuzzolo-Gomez, R. and Greer, R.D. (2004) Emergence of Untaught Mands or Tacts with Novel Adjective-Object Pairs as a Function of Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 30-47.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392995
[37] Maffei, J., Singer-Dudek, S. and Keohane, D. (2014) The Effects of the Establishment of Adult Faces and/or Voices as Conditioned Reinforcers for Children with ASD and Related Disorders. Acta de Investigacion Psicologia, 4, 1621-1641.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S2007-4719(14)70970-6
[38] Du, L., Broto, J. and Greer, R. D. (2015) The Effects of the Establishment of Conditioned Reinforcement for Observing Responses for 3D Stimuli on Generalized Match-to-Sample in Children with Spectrum Disorders. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 16, 82-98.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2015.1065655
[39] Greer, R.D. and Han, H.A.H. (2015) Establishment of Conditioned Reinforcement for Visual Observing and the Emergence of Generalized Visual Identity Matching and Preference for Books with Three Kindergarteners with ASD. Journal of Speech Language Pathology-Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 227-252.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101316
[40] Pereira-Delgado, J.A., Greer, R.D., Speckman, J. and Goswami, A. (2009) Effects of Conditioning Reinforcement for Print Stimuli on Match-to-Sample Responding in Preschoolers. The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis, 3, 199-216.
[41] Greer, R.D., Pistoljevic, N., Cahill, C. and Du, L. (2011) Effects of Conditioning Voices as Reinforcers for Listener Responses on Rate of Learning, Awareness, and Preferences for Listening to Stories in Preschoolers with Autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 103-124.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393095
[42] Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H. and Kuhl, P.K. (2013) Language Experienced in Utero Affects Vowel Perception After Birth: A Two-Country Study. Acta Paediatricia, 102, 156-160.
https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12098
[43] Speckman, J., Longano, J. and Syed, N. (2017) The Effects of Conditioning Three-Dimensional Stimuli on Identity Matching and Imitative Responses in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 22, 111-128.
https://doi.org/10.1037/bdb0000025
[44] Frias, F.A. (2017) How Stimulus Relations Accrue for the Names of Things in Preschoolers (Order No. 10274980). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1896618774).
https://doi.org/10.7916/D8PZ5N6B
[45] Donahoe, J.W. and Palmer, D.C. (2004) Learning and Complex Behavior. Ledgetop Publishing, Richmond.
[46] Gladstone, B.W. and Cooley, J. (1975) Behavioral Similarity as a Reinforcer for Preschool Children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 357-368.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1975.23-357E
[47] Choi, J., Greer, R.D. and Keohane, D.D. (2015) The Effects of an Auditory Match-to-Sample Procedure on Listener and Echoic Responses. The Behavioral Developmental Bulletin, 20, 186-206.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101313
[48] Du, L., Speckman, J., Medina, M. and Cole-Hatchard, M. (2017) The Effects of an Auditory Matching Ipad App on Three Preschoolers’ Advanced Listener Literacy and Echoic Responses. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 10, 1-13.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-017-0174-z
[49] Speckman-Collins, J., Lee Park, H. and Greer, R.D. (2007) Generalized Selection-Based Auditory Matching and the Emergence of The Listener Component of Naming. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention, 4, 412-429.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100382
[50] Greer, R.D., Chavez-Brown, M. Nirgudkar, A.S., Stolfi, L. and Rivera-Valdes, C. (2005) Acquisition of Fluent Listener Responses and the Educational Advancement of Young Children with Autism and Severe Language Delays. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 6, 88-126.
[51] Lee Park, H. (2005) Multiple Exemplar Instruction and Transformation of Stimulus Function from Auditory-Visual Matching to Visual-Visual Matching (Order No. 3174834). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (305017607). http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/305017607?accountid=14258
[52] Miguel, C.F., Carr, J.E. and Michael, J. (2001) The Effects of a Stimulus-Stimulus Pairing Procedure on the Vocal Behavior of Children Diagnosed with Autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 18, 3-13.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392967
[53] Smith, R., Michael, J. and Sundberg, M.L. (1996) Automatic Reinforcement and Automatic Punishment in Infant Vocal Behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 39-48.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392905
[54] Sundberg, M.L., Michael, J., Partington, J.W. and Sundberg, C.A. (1996) The Role of Automatic Reinforcement in Early Language Acquisition. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 21-37.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392904
[55] Yoon, S.Y. and Bennett, G.M. (2000) Effects of a Stimulus-Stimulus Pairing Procedure on Conditioning Vocal Sounds as Reinforcers. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 17, 75-88.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392957
[56] Williams, G. and Greer, R.D. (1993) A Comparison of Verbal-Behavior and Linguistic-Communication Curricula for Training Developmentally Delayed Adolescents to Acquire and Maintain Vocal Speech. Behaviorology, 1, 31-46.
[57] Tsiouri, I., and Greer, R.D. (2003) Inducing Vocal Verbal Behavior in Children with Severe Language Delays through Rapid Motor Imitation Responding. Journal of Behavioral Education, 12, 185-206.
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025508311022
[58] Tsiouri, I. and Greer, R.D. (2007) The Role of Different Social Reinforcement Contingencies in Inducing Echoic Tacts through Motor Imitation Responding in Children with Severe Language Delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 629-647.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100397
[59] Eby, C.M. and Greer, R.D. (2017) Preschoolers’ Initiation of “Spontaneous” Verbal Interactions: Social Reinforcement versus Non-Social Generalized Reinforcers. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 22, 23-43.
https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bdb0000043
[60] Schmelzkopf, J., Greer, R.D., Singer-Dudek, J. and Du, L. (2017) Two Social Learning Conditions to Establish Social Verbal Behavior in Preschoolers. Behavior Development Bulletin.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bdb0000026
[61] Ross, D., Nuzzolo, R., Stolfi, L. and Natarelli, S. (2006) Effects of Speaker Immersion on Independent Speaker Behavior of Preschool Children with Verbal Delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 135-150.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100327
[62] Greer, R.D. and Du, L. (2010) Generic Instruction versus Intensive Tact Instruction and the Emission of Spontaneous Speech. The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 1-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100261
[63] Greenberg, J.H., Tsang, W. and Yip, T. (2014) The Effects of Intensive Tact Instruction with Young Children Having Speech Delays on Pure Tacts and Mands in Non-Instructional Settings: A Partial Replication. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 35-39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100572
[64] Pereira-Delgado, J.A. and Oblak, M. (2007) The Effects of Daily Intensive Tact Instruction on the Emission of Pure Mands and Tacts in Non-Instructional Settings by Three Preschool Children with Developmental Delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4, 392-411.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100381
[65] Pistoljevic, N. and Greer, R.D. (2006) The Effects of Daily Intensive Tact Instruction on Preschool Students’ Emission of Pure Tacts and Mands in Non-Instructional Settings. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 103-120.
http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0100325
[66] Schauffler, G. and Greer, R.D. (2006) The Effects of Intensive Tact Instruction on Audience-Accurate Tacts and Conversational Units. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 120-132.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100326
[67] Farrell, C. (2017) An Investigation into the Speaker-As-Own-Listener Repertoire and Reverse Intraverbal Responding (Order No. 10278214). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1899486948).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1899486948?accountid=14258
[68] Lodhi, S. and Greer, R.D. (1989) The Speaker as Listener. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 353-559.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1989.51-353
[69] Luke, N., Greer, R.D., Singer-Dudek, J. and Keohane, D. (2011) The Emergence of Autoclitic Frames in Atypically and Typically Developing Children as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 141-156.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393098
[70] Speckman-Collins, J. and Greer, R.D. (2012) Multiple Exemplar Instruction and the Emergence of Generative Production of Suffixes as Autoclitic Frames. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28, 83-99.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393109
[71] Lyons, L. (2014) The Effects of the Mastery of Auditory Matching of Component Sounds to Words on the Rate and Accuracy of Textual and Spelling Responses (Order No. 3620326). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1537435883). http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1537435883?accountid=14258
[72] Mackey, M.G. (2017) The Effects of a Reader Immersion Procedure on the Technical Reading Comprehension Responses of Kindergarten and First Grade Students (Order No. 10275628). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1898769193).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1898769193?accountid=14258
[73] Tsai, H. and Greer, R.D. (2006) Conditioned Preference for Books and Faster Acquisition of Textual Responses by Preschool Children. Journal of Early and Inten-sive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 35-60.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0100322
[74] Paniagua, F.A. and Baer, D.M. (1982) The Analysis of Correspondence Training as a Chain Reinforceable at Any Point. Child Development, 786-798.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1129393
[75] Horne, P.J. and Lowe, C.F. (1996) On the Origins of Naming and Other Symbolic Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185-241.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185
[76] Miguel, C. (2016) Common and Intraverbal Bidirectional Naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 125-138.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2
[77] Du, L. and Greer, R.D. (2014) Validation of Adult Generalized Imitation Topographies and the Emergence of Generalized Imitation in Young Children with Autism as a Function of Mirror Training. The Psychological Record, 64, 161-177.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0050-y
[78] Horne, P.J. and Erjavec, M. (2007) Do Infants Show Generalized Imitation of Gestures? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 63-87.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.11-06
[79] Erjavec, M. and Horne, P.J. (2008) Determinants of Imitation of Hand-To-Body Gestures in 2- and 2-Year-Old Children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 183-207.
https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008.89-183
[80] Horne, P.J., Erjavec, M. and Lovett, V.E. (2009) The Effects of Modeling, Local Stimulus Enhancement, and Affordance Demonstration on the Production of Object-Directed Actions in 6-Month-Old Infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 269-281.
https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X297126
[81] Baer, D.M. and Sherman, J.A. (1964) Reinforcement Control of Generalized Imitation in Young Children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1, 37-49.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(64)90005-0
[82] Moreno, J.D. (2012) The Effects of Imitation Instruction Using a Mirror on the Emergence of Duplicative Responses by Preschool Students Diagnosed with Developmental Delays (Order No. 3494688). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (921920595).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/921920595?accountid=14258
[83] Moreno, J., Greer, R.D. and Singer-Dudek, J. (unpublished) The Effects of the Establishment of Generalized Imitation Across Multiple Topographies on Pre-schoolers' Rates of Learning from Demonstration.
[84] Corwin, A. and Greer, R.D. (Unpublished) Effect of the Establishment of Naming on Learning from Teacher-Demonstrations.
[85] Fiorile, C.A. and Greer, R.D. (2007) The Induction of Naming in Children with No Echoic-To-Tact Responses as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71-88.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393048
[86] Longano, J.M. and Greer, R.D. (2014) Is the Source of Naming Multiple Conditioned Reinforcers for Observing Responses? The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 31, 96-117.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-014-0022-y
[87] Lo, C. (2016) How the Presence of The Listener Half of Naming Leads to Multiple Stimulus Control (Order No. 10100022). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1784011549).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1784011549?accountid=14258
[88] Greer, R.D. and Longano, J. (2010) A Rose by Naming: How We May Learn to Do It. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 26, 73-106.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393085
[89] Sidman, M. (2000) Equivalence Relations and the Reinforcement Contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127-146.
http://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127
[90] Bancroft, A.B. (2017) Basic Relational Concept and Verbal Behavior Development in Preschool Children with and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder.
https://doi.org/10.7916/D8TF08VG
[91] Greer, R.D., Singer-Dudek, J. and Gautreaux, G. (2006). Observational Learning. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 486-489.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590500492435
[92] Davies Lackey, A.J. (2005) Yoked Peer Contingencies and the Acquisition of Observational Learning Repertoires (Order No. 3159730). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (305016228).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/305016228?accountid=14258
[93] Gautreaux, G.G. (2005) The Effects of Monitoring Training on the Acquisition of an Observational Learning Repertoire under Peer Tutoring Conditions, Generalization and Collateral Effects (Order No. 3174795). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (305017723).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/305017723?accountid=14258
[94] Heward, W.L. (1994) Three Low-Tech Strategies for Increasing the Frequency of Active Student Response during Group Instruction. In: Garner III, R., Sainato, D.M., Cooper, J.O., Heron, T.E., Heward, W.L., Eshleman, J. and Grossi, T.A., Eds., Behavior Analysis in Education: Focus on Measurably Superior Instruction, Brooks/ Cole, Pacific Grove.
[95] Pereira-Delgado, J.A. and Greer, R.D. (2009) The Effects of Peer Monitoring Training on the Emergence of the Capability to Learn by Observing Instruction Received by Peers. The Psychological Record, 59, 407-434.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395672
[96] Singer-Dudek, J., Choi, J. and Lyons, L. (2013) The Effects of an Observational Intervention on the Emergence of Two Types of Observational Learning. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 14, 329-347.
[97] Stolfi, L. (2005) The Induction of Observational Learning Repertoires in Preschool Children with Developmental Disabilities as a Function of Peer-Yoked Contingencies (Order No. 3174899). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (305016342).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/305016342?accountid=14258
[98] Neu, J. and Greer, R.D. (Unpublished) The Effects of Observation of Learn Units during Reinforcement and Correction Conditions on the Rate of Learning Math Algorithms by Fifth Grade Students.
[99] Donley, C.R. and Greer, R.D. (1993) Setting Events Controlling Social Verbal Exchanges between Students with Developmental Delays. Journal of Behavioral Education, 3, 387-401.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00961542
[100] Baker, K.A. (2014) The Effects of Social Listener Reinforcement and Video Modeling on the Emergence of Social Verbal Operants in Preschoolers Diagnosed with Autism and Language Delays (Order No. 3621578). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1552714267).
http://eduproxy
[101] Sterkin, V.L. (2012) The Effects of The Social Listener Reinforcement Protocol on the Audience Control of Stereotypy and Social Operants for Students with Developmental Delays (Order No. 3508805). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1018548007).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1018548007?accountid=14258
[102] Becker, B.J. (1989) The Effect of Mands and Tacts on Conversational Units and Other Verbal Operants (Order No. 8913097). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (303741088).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/303741088?accountid=14258
[103] Hill-Powell, J. (2015) The Effects of Reading Intervention to Teach Silent Reading with Comprehension on the Speaker-As-Own-Listener Capability among Third Grade Students and Comparisons with Fluent Readers (Order No. 3702441). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1682044938).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1682044938?accountid=14258
[104] Buttigieg, S.F. (2015) The Effects of the Establishment of Conditioned Reinforcement for Observing Books on Rate of Acquisition of Textual Responses With Two-to Four-Year-Old Participants with and without Developmental Delays (Order No. 3684605). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1660540505). http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1660540505?accountid=14258
[105] Cumiskey-Moore, C. (2017) The Effects of Conditioned Reinforcement for Reading on Reading Comprehension for 5th Graders (Order No. 10276197). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1899189458).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1899189458?accountid=14258
[106] Greer, R.D., Saxe, C.D., Becker, B.J. and Mirabella, R.F. (1985) Conditioning Histories and Setting Stimuli Controlling Engagement in Stereotypy or Toy Play. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5, 269-284.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0270-4684(85)90015-1
[107] Greer, R.D., Dorow, L.G., Wachhaus, G. and White, E.R. (1973) Adult Approval and Students’ Music Selection Behavior. Journal of Research in Music Education, 21, 345-354.
https://doi.org/10.2307/3344908
[108] Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M., Rivera, C.M. and Greer, R.D. (2002) Teaching Children with Autism to Prefer Books or Toys Over Stereotypy or Passivity. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 80-87.
https://doi.org/10.1177/109830070200400203
[109] Greer, R.D. (2010) The Verbal Behavior Development Assessment—Revised. CABAS?, New York.
[110] Sidman, M. (1971) Reading and Auditory-Visual Equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5-13.
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1401.05
[111] Mercorella, K.A. (2017) The Effects of Sequencing and Producing Narrative Components of a Story on Reading Comprehension (Order No. 10276191). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1898830456).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1898830456?accountid=14258
[112] Eby, C.M., Greer, R.D., Tullo, L.D., Baker, K.A. and Pauly, R. (2010) Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction on the Transformation of Stimulus Function Across Written and Vocal Spelling Instruction Responses by Students with Autism. The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 20-31.
http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0100262
[113] Helou-Care, Y. (2008) The Effects of the Acquisition of Naming on Reading Comprehension with Academically Delayed Middle School Students Diagnosed with Behavior Disorders (Order No. 3317559). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (304623813).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/304623813?accountid=14258
[114] Helou, Y.J., Lai, J., and Sterkin, V.L. (2007) The Effects of Writer Immersion and the Responses of a Peer Reader on Teaching the Function of Writing with Middle School Students. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4, 483.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100386
[115] Jodlowski, S.M. (2000) The Effects of a Teacher Editor, Peer Editing, and Serving as a Peer Editor on Elementary Students’ Self-Editing Behavior (Order No. 9970212). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (304623374).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/304623374?accountid=14258
[116] Lee, J. (2016) The Effect of a Social Condition on the Establishment of Direct and Indirect Conditioned Reinforcement for Writing by Second Graders (Order No. 10107565). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1791450759). http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1791450759?accountid=14258
[117] Madho, V. (1997) The Effects of the Responses of a Reader on the Writing Effectiveness of Children with Developmental Delays (Order No. 9809740). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (304362197).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/304362197?accountid=14258
[118] Lawson, T.R. and Greer, R.D. (2006) Teaching the Function of Writing to Middle School Students with Academic Delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 3, 151-170.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100328
[119] Weber, J.D. (2016) The Effects of Mastery of Editing Peers’ Written Math Algorithms on Producing Effective Problem Solving Algorithms (Order No. 10108126). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1792625586).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1792625586?accountid=14258
[120] Pellegren, H.E. (2015) The Effects of Mastery of Editing Peers’ Writing on the Functional Writing and Self-Editing Repertoires of Third Graders (Order No. 3702024). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (1681023662).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/1681023662?accountid=14258
[121] Matthews, K.M. (2005) Induction of Metaphorical Responses in Middle School Students as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction (Order No. 3174851). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (305017629).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/305017629?accountid=14258
[122] Meincke, K., Keohane, D.D., Gifaldi, H. and Greer, R.D. (2003) Novel Production of Metaphors as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction. In First European Association for Behavior Analysis Congress, Parma.
[123] Marsico, M.J. (1998) Textual Stimulus Control of Independent Math Performance and Generalization to Reading (Order No. 9822227). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. (304436804).
http://eduproxy.tc-library.org/?url=/docview/304436804?accountid=14258
[124] Bishop, C.D., Erezyilmaz, D.F., Flatt, T., Georgiou, C.D., Hadfield, M.G., Heyland, A., Hodin, J., Jacobs, M.W., Maslakova, S.A., Pires, A., Reitzel, A.M., Santagata, S., Tanaka, K. and Youson, J.H. (2006) What Is Metamorphosis? Integrative and Comparative Biology, 46, 655-661.
https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icl004
[125] West-Eberhard, M.J. (2003) Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
[126] Robinson, G.E. and Barron, A.H. (2017) Epigenetics and the Evolution of Instincts. Science, 356, 26-27.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aam6142
[127] Greer, R.D. (1986) Teaching Operations for Verbal Behavior. CABAS and the Fred S. Keller School, Yonkers, NY.
[128] Greer, R.D., Pohl, P., Du. L. and Moschella, J.L. (Unpublished) Identification and Establishment of Bidirectional Verbal Operants.
[129] Darwin, C.R. (1877) A Biographical Sketch of an Infant. Mind. A Quarterly Review of Psychology and Philosophy, 2, 285-294. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2246907
[130] Darwin, C.R. (1871) The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. Volume 2. John Murray, London.
[131] Geissmann, T. (2002) Duet-Splitting and the Evolution of Gibbon Songs. Biological Reviews, 77, 57-76.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793101005826
[132] Masataka, N. (2007) Music, Evolution and Language. Developmental Science, 10, 35-39.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00561.x
[133] Masataka, N. (2009) The Origin of Language and the Evolution of Music: A Comparative Perspective. Physics of Life Reviews, 6, 11-22.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2008.08.003
[134] Greer, R.D. (2008) The Ontogenetic Selection of Verbal Capabilities: Contributions of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior Theory to a More Comprehensive Understanding of Language. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8, 363-386.
http://oai.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=56080310
[135] Boe, L.-J., Berthommier, F., Legou, T., Captier, G., Kemp, C., Sawallis, T.R., Becker, Y., Rey, A. and Fagot, J. (2017) Evidence of a Vocalic Proto-System in the Baboon (Papio papio) Suggests Pre-Hominin Speech Precursors. PloS One, 12, e0169321
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0169321
[136] Silk, J.B., Seyfarth, R.M. and Cheney, D.L. (2016) Strategic Use of Affiliative Vocalizations by Wild Female Baboons. PLOS ONE, 11, e0163978.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163978
[137] Pohl, P. (1983) Central Auditory Processing. V. Ear Advantages for Acoustic Stimuli in Baboons. Brain and Language, 20, 44-53.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(83)90031-7
[138] Pohl, P., Grubmüller, H.G. and Grubmüller, R. (1984) Developmental Changes in Dichotic Right Ear Advantage (REA). Neuropediatrics, 15, 139-144.
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1052357
[139] Pohl, P. (1984) Ear Advantages for Temporal Resolution in Baboons. Brain and Cognition, 3, 438-444.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(84)90033-2
[140] Studdert-Kennedy, M. and Shankweiler, D. (1970) Hemispheric Specialization for Speech Perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 579-594.
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1912174
[141] Konishi, M. (1985) Birdsong: from Behavior to Neuron. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 8, 125-170.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.001013
[142] Nottebohm, F. (2005) The Neural Basis of Birdsong. PLoS Biology, 3, e164.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030164
[143] Marler, P. (1970) Birdsong and Speech Development: Could There Be Parallels? American Scientist, 58, 669-673.
http://www.jstor.org/stable/27829317
[144] Marler, P. (1970) A Comparative Approach to Vocal Learning: Song Development in White-Crowned Sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 1-25.
http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0029144
[145] Soha, J.A. and Peters, S. (2015) Vocal Learning in Songbirds and Humans: A Retrospective in Honor of Peter Marler. Ethology, 121, 933-945.
https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12415
[146] Tattersall, I. (2000) Once We Were Not Alone. Scientific American, 282, 56-62.
https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0100-56
[147] Beck, U. (2016) The Metamorphosis of the World. John Wiley & Sons (Polity), Cambridge, UK.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.