Music Modulates the Strength of Vection

Abstract

We presented four types of music (two fast tempo and two slow tempo types) during illusory self-motion perception (vection). Vection was induced by expansional dots (optic flow), and participants estimated its strength via magnitude estimation and by pressing a button. Our purpose of this study is to examine whether music alter vection strength. Results showed that vection was facilitated by two fast tempo types and one slow tempo type of music in lieu of a no-music condition. We speculated that fast tempo, active music might induce higher arousal levels in participants compared with a no-music condition, and that higher arousal levels might induce stronger vection. We speculated that this auditory method of modulating vection strength can be utilized in the virtual reality environment.


Share and Cite:

Seno, T. (2013). Music Modulates the Strength of Vection. Psychology, 4, 566-568. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.47081.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Brodsky, W. (2002). The effects of music tempo on simulated driving performance and vehicular control. Transportation Research, 4, 219-241.
[2] Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In P. N. Juslin, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 431-449). New York: Oxford University Press.
[3] Herrington, J. D., & Capella, L. M. (1996). Effects of music in service environments: A field study. The Journal of Services Marketing, 10, 26-41. doi:10.1108/08876049610114249
[4] Husain, G., Thompson, W. F., & Schellengerg, E. G. (2002). Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities. Music Perception, 20, 151-171. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.151
[5] Ihaya, K., Seno, T., & Yamada, Y. (submitted) Più mosso: Fast self-motion accelerates mental tempo in virtual reality.
[6] Iwamiya, S. (1997) Interaction between auditory and visual processing in car audio: Simulation experiment using video reproduction. Applied Human Science, 16, 115-119. doi:10.2114/jpa.16.115
[7] Miell, D. E., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2005). Musical communication. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[8] Mayfield, C., & Moss, S. (1989). Effect of music tempo on task performance. Psychological Reports, 65, 1283-1290. doi:10.2466/pr0.1989.65.3f.1283
[9] Milliman, R. (1982). Using background music to affect the behaviour of supermarket shoppers. Journal of Marketing, 46, 86-91. doi:10.2307/1251706
[10] Milliman, R. (1986). The influence of background music on behaviour of restaurant patrons. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 286-289. doi:10.1086/209068
[11] Nittono, H., Tsuda, A., Akai, S., & Nakajima, Y. (2000). Tempo of background sound and performance speed. Perception and Motor Skills, 90, 1122. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.90.3c.1122
[12] Seno, T., Kawabe T., Ito. H., & Sunaga, S. (2013) Vection modulates emotional valence of autobiographical episodic memories. Cognition, 126, 115-120. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.009
[13] Spinney, L. (1997) Pump down the volume. New Scientist, 155, 22.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.