Association of Chronic Stress with Agency, Theory of Mind Function and Abstract Construal in Women

Abstract

Stress disrupts the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) networks required for consciousness, abstract construal, self-referential thinking, reward circuitry and theory-of-mind functions. We used stress assessment inventories to survey a cohort of 113 women for self-reported stress over a 5 - 10-year period. We identified two subsets of women consistently reporting either high (n = 21) or low (n = 24) stress over this period. These cohorts answered the eSAIL, REM and Apathy Evaluation Scale questionnaires. Significant differences were found between cohorts in apathy and REM scores, as well as in RD, self-agency and abstraction, but not in other subscales of the eSAIL.

Share and Cite:

Mascarenhas, D. (2016) Association of Chronic Stress with Agency, Theory of Mind Function and Abstract Construal in Women. Psychology, 7, 1397-1401. doi: 10.4236/psych.2016.712140.

1. Introduction

Although recent work has begun to address psychometric correlates of innovation and entrepreneurship (Mascarenhas, Singh, Singh, & Veer, 2007; Mascarenhas & Singh, 2012; Mascarenhas & Veer, 2014; Steel, Rinne, & Fairweather, 2012; Geels, 2004) , the real-life impact of chronic stress on the underlying cognitive and motivational variables is not well understood. Stress affects core circuits in the medial prefrontal cortex (Goldwater, Pavlides, Hunter, Bloss, Hof, McEwen, & Morrison, 2009; Shansky & Morrison, 2009) that subserve consciousness (Marino, Bonanno, & Giorgio, 2015) , theory- of-mind functions (Baetens, Ma, Steen, & Van Overwalle, 2014; Isoda & Noritake, 2013) , humor (Franklin & Adams, 2011) , level of construal (Gilead, Liberman, & Maril, 2014) , intentional stance (Spunt, Meyer, & Lieberman, 2015) and reward valence assessment (Hogeveen, Hauner, Chau, Krueger, & Grafman, 2016) .

As a control for agency, we use a reciprocal scale, the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES), which has been previously described (Clarke, Reekum, Simard, Streiner, Freedman, & Conn, 2007; Yuen, Gunning, & Woods, 2014) . To measure affective theory-of-mind functions, we use the widely employed and previously validated REM test (Olderbak, Wilhelm, Olaru, Geiger, Brenneman, & Roberts, 2015) .

Research Aims

This work aims to measure the impacts of chronic stress on psychometric measures relevant to innovation and entrepreneurship while helping construct a hypothetical framework for future neuropsychological research in this area.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and Settings

A subset of a previously reported cohort was used in this study. All participants provided informed consent and the study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board. Procedures for online data collection from the original 1277 anonymous adult participants have been described in three previously published studies using the eSAIL, a 43-item online inventory for which satisfactory Cronbach alpha and test-retest reliability coefficients have been observed for all component psychometric scales, while discriminant, convergent, and predictive validities have been shown across cohorts (Mascarenhas, Singh, Singh, & Veer, 2007) .

2.2. Procedure

113 individuals from the original cohort who scored high (>0.3 SD above mean) or low (<0.3 SD below mean) scores in the STRESS subscale of the eSAIL in 2005 or 2010 additionally answered the STRESS-6 in 2014 and 2015, and the REM, AES and AGENCY inventories in 2015. Two subsets of women consistently reporting either high (n = 21) or low (n = 24) stress in all three stress inventories over either the 5 or 10-year period were combined for data analysis.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. STRESS-6 Questionnaire

The STRESS-6 questionnaire is a six-item subset of a previously validated stress inventory (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) .

2.3.2. eSAIL and Subscales Thereof

2.3.3. REM Test

To measure affective theory-of-mind functions, the widely used and previously described and validated REM test was employed (Olderbak, Wilhelm, Olaru, Geiger, Brenneman, & Roberts, 2015) .

2.4. Data Analysis

Data are presented as means ± SD. Probability values (p values) were computed using Student’s t-test.

3. Results

Table 1 shows a number of significant differences between the two cohorts. As expected, the Persistent High Stress group scored significantly higher than the Persistent Low Stress group in the STRESS subscale of the eSAIL, as well as in the STRESS-6 inventory.

Table 1. Chronic stress and average Z scores#.

*Subscale of the eSAIL; #Population mean = 0.

In contrast, the IMPROMPTU and MACH subscale scores of the eSAIL showed no significant differences between groups.

4. Discussion

4.1. Chronic Stress and Psychometric Data

4.2. Implications and Limitations

This is a preliminary finding in a small cohort. A larger study will be needed to confirm these initial observations. The practical implications of this finding, if confirmed by future studies, are potentially significant for innovation management. The link between psychometric scales and innovation is supported by published data (Mascarenhas, Singh, Singh, & Veer, 2007; Mascarenhas & Singh, 2012; Mascarenhas & Veer, 2014; Steel, Rinne, & Fairweather, 2012; Geels, 2004) . Taken together, these facts may suggest the increasing importance of resilience to stress in the management of knowledge economy within which shorter product cycles create a growing dependence on innovation.

Cite this paper

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Baetens, K., Ma, N., Steen, J., & Van Overwalle, F. (2014). Involvement of the Mentalizing Network in Social and Non-Social High Construal. Social Cognitive Affect Neuroscience, 9, 817-824.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst048
[2] Clarke, D. E., Reekum, R., Simard, M., Streiner, D. L., Freedman, M., & Conn, D. (2007). Apathy in Dementia: An Examination of the Psychometric Properties of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 19, 57-64.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/jnp.2007.19.1.57
[3] Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-396.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2136404
[4] Franklin Jr., R. G., & Adams Jr., R.B. (2011). The Reward of a Good Joke: Neural Correlates of Viewing Dynamic Displays of Stand-Up Comedy. Cognitive Affect Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 508-515.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0049-7
[5] Geels, F. W. (2004). From Sectoral Systems of Innovation to Socio-Technical Systems: Insights about Dynamics and Change from Sociology and Institutional Theory. Research Policy, 33, 897-920.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.respol.2004.01.015
[6] Gilead, M., Liberman, N., & Maril, A. (2014). From Mind to Matter: Neural Correlates of Abstract and Concrete Mindsets. Social Cognitive Affect Neuroscience, 9, 638-645.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst031
[7] Goldwater, D. S., Pavlides, C., Hunter, R. G., Bloss, E. B., Hof, P. R., McEwen, B. S., & Morrison, J. H. (2009). Structural and Functional Alterations to Rat Medial Prefrontal Cortex Following Chronic Restraint Stress and Recovery. Neuroscience, 164, 798-808.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.053
[8] Hogeveen, J., Hauner, K. K., Chau, A., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2016). Impaired Valuation Leads to Increased Apathy Following Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Damage. Cerebral Cortex, bhv317. First Published Online January 5, 2016.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv317
[9] Isoda, M., & Noritake, A. (2013). What Makes the Dorsomedial Frontal Cortex Active during Reading the Mental States of Others? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 232-240.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00232
[10] Marino, S., Bonanno, L., & Giorgio, A. (2016). Functional Connectivity in Disorders of Consciousness: Methodological Aspects and Clinical Relevance. Brain Imaging Behavior, 10, 604-608.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9417-1
[11] Mascarenhas, D., Singh, B. K., Singh, A.H., & Veer, S. V. (2007). Early Adoption of New Drug Treatments: The Role of Continuing Medical Education and physician Adaptivity. Critical Pathways in Cardiology, 6, 30-40.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.hpc.0000257844.53130.d0
[12] Mascarenhas, D., & Singh, A. H. (2012). Regional Culture and Adaptive Behavior of Physicians. Journal of Bioeconomics, 14, 257-266.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10818-011-9115-z
[13] Mascarenhas, D. D., & Veer, S. V. (2014). Women, Innovation and Literature. Journal of Innovation and Entrepreneurship, 3, 7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/2192-5372-3-7
[14] Olderbak, S., Wilhelm, O., Olaru, G., Geiger, M., Brenneman, M. W., & Roberts, R. D. (2015). A Psychometric Analysis of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test: Toward a Brief Form for Research and Applied Settings. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1503-1508.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01503
[15] Shansky, R. M., & Morrison, J. H. (2009). Stress-Induced Dendritic Remodeling in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex: Effects of Circuit, Hormones and Rest. Brain Research, 1293, 108-113.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.062
[16] Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., & Lieberman, M. D. (2015). The Default Mode of Human Brain Function Primes the Intentional Stance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1116-1124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00785
[17] Steel, G. D., Rinne, T., & Fairweather, J. (2012). Personality, Nations, and Innovation: Relationships between Personality Traits and National Innovation Scores. Cross-Cultural Research, 46, 3-8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397111409124
[18] Yuen, G. S., Gunning, F. M., & Woods, E. (2014). Neuroanatomical Correlates of Apathy in Late-Life Depression and Antidepressant Treatment Response. Journal of Affective Disorders, 166, 179-186.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.008

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.