Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science
Vol.05 No.03(2015), Article ID:54939,11 pages
10.4236/jbbs.2015.53011

Hippocampal Pallium and Map-Like Memories through Vertebrate Evolution

C. Broglio, I. Martín-Monzón, F. M. Ocaña, A. Gómez, E. Durán, C. Salas, F. Rodríguez*

Laboratory of Psychobiology. University of Sevilla, Campus Santiago Ramón y Cajal, Sevilla, Spain

Email: *fernanr@us.es

Academic Editor: Juan J. Canales, University of Leicester, UK

Copyright © 2015 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY).

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Received 13 February 2015; accepted 21 March 2015; published 24 March 2015

ABSTRACT

The hippocampus in humans and other mammals is essential for episodic and relational memories. Comparative evidence indicates that a hippocampal pallium homologue is present in birds, reptiles, amphibians, ray-finned fishes, cartilaginous fishes and agnathans. Some of their characteristics, such as the topological position and the pattern of connectivity, appear remarkably well conserved. We review here substantial data showing that in all the vertebrate groups studied up to date, from fish to mammals, the hippocampus plays a fundamental role in spatial memory. In these vertebrates groups, the hippocampal pallium homologue is involved in the use of map-like, relational representations of the objective space that provide stable allocentric frames of reference, thus allowing flexible navigation. These similarities suggest a common evolutionary ancestry and indicate that the functional properties of the hippocampus appear early in the vertebrate phylogenesis and are retained through the independent evolution of the vertebrate lineages.

Keywords:

Hippocampus, Spatial Memory, Amniotes, Anamniotes, Vertebrate Brain Evolution

1. Introduction

Several decades of neuroscientific and neuropsychological research put forward that the human hippocampus serves a fundamental and selective role in our capacity to recall everyday facts and events [1] -[5] . The hippocampus, the most medial division of the telencephalic pallium, is critically involved in the encoding and recollection of unique personal experiences (episodic or autobiographical memories), and in linking episodes by their common features into relational networks (relational memories) that support flexible memory expression. Spatial memories are also a particular form of relational memory intimately linked to the hippocampal function. Indeed, some particular forms of cognitive representations of space are considered the clearest animal equivalent to human declarative memory, and their comparative study has provided essential clues that significantly have contributed to our understanding of the hippocampal functional traits [6] - [10] . We review here data indicating that spatial cognition abilities in different groups of vertebrates are remarkably similar, and can rely on homologous neural mechanisms. Map-like spatial memories are not an exclusive attribute of mammals; these sophisticated cognitive capabilities are also present in birds, reptiles and teleost fish. In addition, neurobehavioral studies show that map-like spatial memories in these vertebrate groups depend on the hippocampal pallium.

2. Medial Pallium or Hippocampus and Map-Like Memories in Amniotes

Available data show that mammals and birds are able to use allocentric or “world centered” representations for navigation and goal location. These map-like, high order representations of the objective space (cognitive maps) provide a stable spatial frame of reference independent of the subject’s own position, thus permitting the efficient use of spatial information to navigate to a goal [10] . Cognitive mapping allows the subject to learn the location of the goal by encoding its reciprocal metrical and geometrical spatial relationships with a variety of environmental features and gives rise to distinctive behavioral features, for example, resistance to partial losses of relevant information, and flexible detouring and shortcutting [9] - [12] . In mammals and birds, allocentric, map- like spatial representations depend on the hippocampal formation. Neuropsychological and lesion studies show that damage to the hippocampus and associated structures causes selective deficits in the capacity for place learning and memory, but not when reaching the goal can be solved by alternative strategies such as approaching a single landmark (cue learning) or by non-spatial discriminations [8] [10] [13] - [18] . A number of recent thorough behavioral studies provide strong evidence indicating that also reptiles, in particular turtles, lizards and snakes, display sophisticated spatial behavior based on map-like, allocentric or relational spatial representations of the environment [19] - [26] , and that these cognitive capabilities depend on the medial cortex, the reptilian pallial region considered homologous to the avian and mammalian hippocampal formation on the basis of embryological, anatomical and neurophysiological evidence [27] - [32] .

The medial cortex (MC) of reptiles is organized in three layers and is composed of a medial and a dorsomedial subdivision. The medial, small-celled, and the dorsomedial, large celled subdivisions are thought to be homologous to the dentate gyrus and the hippocampus proper, respectively [33] . Moreover, the connectivity pattern of the MC subdivisions resembles that of the hippocampal formation. Like the dentate gyrus, the medial subdivision of MC of reptiles has reciprocal connections with the dorsomedial subdivision of MC and the dorsal cortex and receives inputs from the olfactory cortex. The dorsomedial subdivision of MC, like the hippocampus, is primarily connected with the medial subdivision of MC and with the contralateral dorsomedial subdivision of MC by a substantial commissural pathway. In addition, the MC of reptiles is reciprocally connected with the septal nuclei and receives inputs from the diencephalon (dorsomedial nucleus of the dorsal thalamus, mammillary nuclei and periventricular hypothalamus) and the brain stem (raphe and locus coeruleus nuclei and reticular formation) [32] - [35] .

Experimental evidence indicates that the medial cortex of reptiles, like the hippocampus in mammals and birds, plays a central role in spatial cognition. In previous studies we obtained evidence indicating that MC lesions in turtles [36] - [38] produce deficits in a variety of allocentric, place learning tasks but not in cue learning and other egocentrically referenced learning tasks (Figure 1). For example, MC lesions produce a severe and selective place memory deficit in turtles trained in a dry-maze analogue, as indicated by the post-surgery failure to navigate to the goal [36] . In addition, MC lesioned turtles trained in a standard four-arm maze place task suffer a place-learning impairment as revealed by their failure to reach the goal during test trials. In fact, these animals lose the ability to navigate to the goal when they start from novel positions, and when the visual cues in its proximity are excluded [37] . These results indicate that MC lesioned turtles lack the capacity to encode goal location relative to the environmental features in a relational, map-like representation (place learning). MC lesions in turtles also produce deficits in the reversal learning of a spatial task, which is a characteristic effect of hippocampal lesions in mammals and birds [38] . Moreover, similar to hippocampus lesions, MC lesions in turtles do not impair, or even facilitate, the use of guidance and other non-relational, egocentric strategies to reach the goal [36] [37] [39] . That is, the effects of damage in the MC of turtles and in the hippocampus of mammals and birds

Figure 1. Spatial learning deficits in medial cortex (MC) lesioned turtles trained in allocentric spatial tasks. Lesioned animals were trained in place and cue tasks, and probe and transfer tests were conducted after the animals reached the acquisition criterion. A Mean percentages of correct choices for the MC and Sh turtles trained in a place or a cue task in a four-arm maze placed in the centre of a room with abundant distal visual cues. The diagrams on the left show a schematic representation of the spatial and the cue procedures. In the place task the goal (grey circle) was maintained in the same place of the room throughout the experiment, but two start positions were used randomly (50% each); in the cue task a conspicuous intramaze visual cue (checked panel) signaled directly the location of the goal, but the position of the cue (goal) varied in a pseudorandom order from trial to trial; in addition, two start positions were used randomly (50% each). MC-lesioned turtles in the place task failed to locate the goal during transfer test trials in which new start positions were used, whereas sham animals navigated directly to the goal independently of start location. In contrast, no deficit was observed in cue learning. The maze diagrams on the right show the trajectories chosen by the turtles in the place group in the test trials in which the maze was displaced within the room in such a way that novel start locations, never used during training, were used. The gray circles show the goal location during training. Note that during these test trials the end of one maze arm always coincided with the goal place. The position of the maze during training is shown in dotted lines. The numbers and the relative thickness of the arrows denote the percentage of times that a particular choice was made. Note that the Sh animals are able to navigate towards the goal spontaneously using novel routes from different directions, demonstrating mapping abilities. Instead, MC-lesioned turtles trained in the place procedure showed a dramatic impairment during test trials in which new start positions were used. The brain drawings show a schematic representation of the largest (gray shading) and smallest (black shading) extent of the MC lesions, reconstructed in transversal sections. Abbreviations: ca, central nucleus of the amygdala; cn, core nucleus of dvr; d, area d; dc, dorsal cortex; dvr, dorsal ventricular ridge; gp, globus pallidus; lnot, lateral nucleus of the olfactory tract; ma, medial nucleus of the amygdala; mc, medial cortex; ot, optic tract; pa, paleostriatum augmentatum; pc, pyriform cortex; ph, primordium hippocampi; pv, periventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus; pt, pallial thickening; r, nucleus rotundus; v, ventricle. Modified from Ref. [37] . B MC lesions produce a dramatic impairment in the implementation of allocentric spatial strategies in an open field maze task with four feeders, where only one was baited (goal). The bars show the mean number of errors to reach the goal in the post-criterion training trials, and in the probe test in which the proximal visual cues were removed, and in the transfer test in which new start positions where used. The removal of the cues was designed to test the ability of the MC lesioned turtles to reach the goal regardless of the partial loss of relevant environmental information, a distinctive property of cognitive mapping. Note that MC-lesioned animals failed to reach the goal when the cues in its proximity were excluded or when new start positions were employed, indicating that they used a guidance strategy to solve the task. The diagrams on the right show representative searching trajectories from the start (arrow) to the goal feeder (black circle) on the transfer trials in which new start positions were employed. Note that MC-lesioned turtles, in contrast to sham operated (Sh) animals, failed to reach the goal when new start positions were employed. Asterisks denote significant statistical differences (p < 0.05). Modified from Ref. [36] .

are strikingly similar not only in relation to the functions that are impaired, but also in relation to the processes that are not affected or even facilitated.

Neuro ecological works in Squamata (lizards and snakes) have shown that the more intensive use of space correlates positively with larger MC volume and higher neuronal plasticity. Free-ranging northern Pacific rattlesnakes (Crotalus o. oreganus) that return to their home area after experimental displacements have larger MC relative to the dorsal cortex, compared with hand manipulated or undisturbed snakes [40] . Interspecific comparisons in closely related species of lizards have revealed that species with higher demands on spatial abilities have increased hippocampal volume. Thus, the MC in Acanthodactylus boskianus, an active forager, is larger than in A. scutellatus, a sit-and-wait predator [41] [42] . In the snake Agkistrodon piscivorus, males, that have larger home ranges than females, present a larger MC as well as increased neurogenesis and cell migration relative to the dorsal cortex [43] . Territorial males of side-blotched lizards (Uta stansburiana) that inhabit broader areas have increased MC neurogenesis rate when compared with those living in smaller (spatially impoverished) areas [44] . Adult red-sided garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis), that present seasonal physiological and behavioral variations, have increased neurogenesis and cell migration in the MC associated with migratory rhythms [45] . In addition, although experimental data are scarce, medial pallium dependent learning and memory processes in reptiles are likely based on conserved molecular mechanisms. For example, as observed in mammals and birds, both NMDA-dependent and NMDA-independent long-term potentiation have been found in the MC of turtles [46] [47] .

Hence, the presence of medial cortex-dependent allocentric spatial learning and memory capabilities in reptiles, birds, and mammals suggests that this trait could be a primitive character in amniotes. As the amniotes appear to be a monophyletic group that diverged from primitive tetrapods during the early Carboniferous, it is reasonable to assume that the hippocampus-dependent cognitive mapping memory is an ancient brain-behavior property present, at least in the stem reptilian ancestor of modern amniotes, retained through the independent evolution of each extant lineage.

3. Map-Like Memories and Hippocampal Pallium in Anamniotes

Developmental, neuroanatomical and neurophysiological evidence indicates that a pallial region homologous to the hippocampus of amniotes is also present in the forebrain of amphibians, lungfishes, ray-finned fishes, cartilaginous fishes and agnathans [27] [31] , suggesting that a hippocampal pallium is a primitive feature of the vertebrate telencephalon. However, studies on spatial cognition capabilities are very scarce or even completely lacking in some anamniote groups.

Although the studies on the spatial capabilities of amphibians are clearly insufficient, recent laboratory and field experiments suggest that they possess elaborated spatial abilities [48] [49] . Also in Chondrichthyes or cartilaginous fish data concerning their spatial capabilities are really scarce, but a few experimental and field studies show that sharks (elasmobranchs) can use different strategies for navigation, and have long-term spatial memory [50] - [52] . No data are available relative to this issue in agnathans. Regrettably, there are not, to our knowledge, studies on the role of the medial pallium in spatial cognition in amphibians, cartilaginous fish and agnathans. This lack of information is particularly notable given the basal position of these groups in the vertebrate phylogenetic tree (Figure 2) and their significance for understanding the evolution of the hippocampal pallium and spatial cognition.

The greatest part of the available information on anamniotes is concentrated in ray-finned (actinopterygian) fishes. The telencephalon of these fishes constitutes a special case in vertebrates, as a result of a major divergence during the early embryogenesis [53] - [56] (Figure 3). In actinopterygians, the telencephalon develops by a

Figure 2. Cladogram illustrating the phylogenetic relationships among the main groups of extant vertebrates. The drawings show a dorsal view of the brain of representative species of each group.

Figure 3. Schematic representation of the process of evagination and inversion that occurs in the telencephalon of non-actinopterygian vertebrates during embryonic development compared with the process of eversion or bending outward that occurs in actinopterygian fish. In tetrapods and many fishes, the evagination of the dorsolateral prosencephalic wall produces paired telencephalic hemispheres with internal ventricles. In actinopterygian fish, the dorsal portion of the embryonic prosencephalic wall curves laterally, producing two massive hemispheres flanking a single ventricular cavity. The photographs at the right show a transverse section through the telencephalon of a species representative of the evaginated forebrain (turtle, Pseudemys scripta) and another representative of the everted forebrain (goldfish, Carassius auratus). P1, P2, and P3 correspond to the three main subdivisions of the pallium. Abbreviations: DC, dorsal cortex; DMC, dorsomedial cortex; Dld, dorsal subdivision of lateral division of area dorsalis; Dlv, ventral subdivision of lateral division of area dorsalis; Dm, medial subdivision of area dorsalis; DVR, dorsal ventricular ridge; LC, lateral cortex; MC, medial cortex; Pt, pallial thickening; tc, tela choroidea; v, ventricle.

process of eversion (bending outward of the embryonic prosencephalic alar plate) instead of evagination as occurs in every other vertebrate group. As a consequence, the telencephalic hemispheres of ray-finned fishes lack internal ventricles and the medial-to-lateral topography of the pallial areas is reversed compared with that observed in every other vertebrate. Accordingly, the actinopterygian pallial area considered homologous to the hippocampus is located laterally in the telencephalon [55] [57] - [60] .

Among ray-finned fishes, the teleosts are the most intensively studied group concerning the telencephalic anatomical and functional organization and the neural bases of their spatial capabilities. The topography, connections and histochemistry of the dorsolateral pallium (Dl) of teleost fish are remarkably similar to the hippocampal pallium of vertebrates with a developmentally evaginated telencephalon [27] [59] [61] . For example, Dl has widespread reciprocal connections with other pallial areas, and with the contralateral Dl. It is also reciprocally connected with the ventral nucleus of the area ventralis (Vv), considered homologous to the septal nuclei of amniotes [27] , from which it receives a cholinergic input, and with the preglomerular complex of the posterior tuberculum (the possible homologue of the dorsal thalamus), from which it receives multi-sensory inputs. Dl also projects to the preoptic area and other diencephalic regions and receives inputs from the preoptic area, the locus coeruleus and the superior raphe [61] . However, within Dl, the ventral subdivision (Dlv) is the most likely candidate as the specific homologue of the tetrapod medial pallium, because of its distal topological position in the pallium, its pattern of connectivity with subcortical areas, and by the distribution pattern of several histochemical and molecular markers, for example, the dopamine receptor subtype D1B, which is characteristic of the mammalian hippocampus [62] . However, it is still under discussion whether the entire Dl, i.e. the dorsal subdivision (Dld) plus Dlv, or by contrast Dlv exclusively, are comparable to the hippocampal pallium. In fact, some hypotheses propose Dld as a part of the dorsal pallium or isocortex based on its topological position, its visual ascending inputs, and its visually related activity [63] - [65] . In contrast, other hypotheses, based on a topological criterion and the presumable similarity in the pattern of connections of Dld and Dlv, propose that the whole Dl should be viewed as a single primary pallial division, homologous to the mammalian hippocampus [61] [66] .

Our previous studies on goldfish show that teleost fish, like land vertebrates, are able to navigate on basis of relational, map-like cognitive representations that provide world-centered, allocentric frames of spatial reference [67] - [75] . These representations, based on the encoding of multiple environmental features such as the objects and their reciprocal spatial (topological and geometrical) relationships, allow them to navigate accurately and flexibly to a goal regardless of their own actual position and local view, and use spontaneously shortcuts and detours (Figure 4). Moreover, data provided by lesion and morphofunctional studies indicate that the Dlv pallial area of teleost fishes, like the hippocampus of land vertebrates, selectively supports these spatial cognition abilities.

Dlv lesions produce dramatic learning and memory impairments in goldfish trained in tasks that require the use of allocentric spatial strategies [37] [76] [77] (Figure 4). Dlv lesions cause place-memory deficits in goldfish trained in a four-arm maze surrounded by distal visual cues [37] , making the lesioned animals unable to navigate to familiar food-rewarded locations, and to implement novel routes to the goal when departing from new start positions (Figure 4A). Moreover, when goldfish with lesions in the main pallial divisions are trained in an open-field place task or in a spatial constancy task, only Dlv lesioned fish are unable to implement allocentric strategies [76] [77] . For example, Dlv lesioned fish are impaired in learning the reversal of the spatial constancy task (Figure 4B), and fail to reach the goal when the cues in its proximity are excluded (Figure 4D). By contrast damage to Dlv does not impair the use of guidance or other egocentric strategies [37] [76] [77] (Figure 4A and Figure 4B). These spatial learning and memory deficits are similar to those observed in mammals and birds following lesions in the hippocampal formation [8] [14] - [17] [78] .

Studies using complementary techniques also reveal the critical role of the teleost Dlv in spatial cognition. Training goldfish in the spatial constancy task (but not in a cued version of the same task) produces an increase in the synthesis of proteins that is selective to the neurons of Dlv [76] [79] (Figure 4C), as indicated by the enlargement of the nucleolar organizing regions (the organelles associated with the synthesis of ribosomal proteins) [80] . Cytochrome oxidase histochemistry, an approach that permits to determine the brain regions undergoing long-term training-dependent metabolic changes [81] - [84] , shows that training goldfish in the spatial constancy task increases the activity in Dlv but not in Dld [85] . Consistent data obtained in neuroecological studies show that increased spatial behavior demands correlate with larger Dlv in several species of teleost fishes [86] [87] . For example, in the Azorean rock-pool blennies (Parablennius parvicornis), the volume of the Dlv is larg-

Figure 4. Involvement of the goldfish ventral subdivision of the dorsolateral pallium (Dlv) in map-like memories. A Dlv lesions abolish place learning in goldfish tested in a four-arm maze placed in the centre of a room with abundant distal cues. The curves show the mean percentage of correct choices in a place and a cue tasks by goldfish with dorsomedial pallium (Dm) or Dlv lesions, or sham-operated (Sh). The insets above the curves show a schematic representation of the spatial and the cue procedures. The presurgery performance in the place task and the results in the transfer trials indicate that goldfish are able to build complex spatial representations of their environment and to solve spatial tasks on the basis of allocentric frames of reference. Following surgery, Dlv-lesioned animals were severely impaired in the solution of the task, whereas Dm-lesioned animals did not decrease accuracy relative to sham (Sh) animals. The maze diagrams on the right show the trajectories chosen by Sh and Dlv-lesioned goldfish during the transfer trials conducted after surgery, when the maze was displaced in the room. Note that always the end of one arm was located in the place where the fish was rewarded during training trials, but the start positions and the trajectories were different. The numbers and the relative thickness of the arrows denote the percentage of times that a particular choice was made. The position of the maze during training trials is shown by dotted lines. The gray circles show the goal location during training. Note that during these trials Sh goldfish consistently chose the route leading to the place where they were rewarded during training trials, indicating their ability to use new pathways towards a goal from unfamiliar start points, and to reorganize their spatial strategies in response to an environmental change. In contrast, the random distribution of choices by the Dlv-lesioned animals indicates a severe spatial deficit. The brain drawings in the bottom left show a schematic representation of the largest (dark gray) and smallest (light gray) extent of the Dlv and Dm lesions, reconstructed in transversal sections at the levels indicated in the lateral view of the brain. Modified from Ref. [37] . B Dlv lesions affect the retention (left) and the reversal learning (right) of a spatial constancy task which requires the use of allocentric or relational strategies. The insets show schematic representations of the procedure. Modified from Ref. [76] . C Training in the spatial constancy task produces a selective increment in the transcriptive activity of Dlv neurons, as indicated by increases in the size of the nucleolar organizing region (NOR), the nucleolar organelles associated with the synthesis of ribosomal proteins. Note that training in the cue version of the same procedure did not produce observable changes. The photograph on the right shows AgNORs (argyrophilic nucleolar organizing regions) in Dlv neurons. Dlv-d and Dlv-v correspond to the dorsal and ventral subdivisions of Dlv, respectively. Modified from Ref. [76] . D Dlv, but not Dm lesions, produces a dramatic impairment in the implementation of allocentric spatial strategies in a “hole-board” analogue task. In this task animals are required to find the baited feeder within a 5 × 5 feeder matrix surrounded by visual cues. The bars show the mean spatial accuracy index (in this index values range from 1 for the goal feeder to 0 for the most distant one) for each group during the training trials conducted after reaching the acquisition criterion and in the probe tests in which the most distal or proximal cues to the goal were removed. The diagrams on the right show representative searching trajectories from the start to the goal feeder (gray circle) on the probe trials for a representative animal of each group. Note that Dlv-lesioned goldfish failed to reach the goal when the cues in its proximity were excluded indicating that they used a guidance strategy to solve the task. Asterisks denote significant statistical differences (p < 0.05). Modified from Ref. [77] .

er in females than in males, associated with a more demanding use of space during the breeding season [86] . In a whole, these data strongly suggest that the Dlv of the teleost telencephalic pallium is critically involved in allocentric spatial learning and memory and that, like the hippocampus of amniotes, it is an essential component of a memory system that processes and encodes environmental information in complex, high-order relational memory representations.

4. Conclusion

Comparative neuroanatomical, developmental and functional data reveal that the evolution of the vertebrate forebrain could have been more conservative than previously realized. Despite conspicuous morphological and

cytoarchitectural differences, the pallium of vertebrates seems to be organized in comparable subdivisions and a hippocampal pallium homologue seems to be present in every vertebrate radiation [29] [30] [60] [63] [88] - [92] . From agnathans to mammals, the hippocampal pallium of all extant vertebrates shares a suite of developmental, neurochemical and connectivity characteristics, as well as functional properties, which can indicate a common evolutionary ancestry. Although additional studies in basal vertebrate groups are needed to understand the evo- lution of the allocentric navigation systems, it is reasonable to assume that some basic organizational and functional properties of the hippocampal pallium emerged early during the vertebrate phylogenesis, and that these properties are conserved during the independent evolution of the different vertebrate radiations. These conserved properties probably explain the widespread role of the hippocampal pallium in spatial cognition.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grant PSI2011-27489 from the Spanish MINECO and F.E.D.E.R.

References

  1. Aggleton, J.P. and Brown, M.W. (1999) Episodic Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal-Anterior Thalamic Axis. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22, 425-489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002034
  2. Burgess, N., Maguire, E.A. and O’Keefe, J. (2002) The Human Hippocampus and Spatial and Episodic Memory. Neuron, 35, 625-641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9
  3. Eichenbaum, H. and Cohen, N.J. (2001) From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain. Oxford University Press, New York.
  4. Konkel, A. and Cohen, N.J. (2009) Relational Memory and the Hippocampus: Representations and Methods. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3, 166-174. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.023.2009
  5. Squire, L.R., Stark, C.E. and Clark, R.E. (2004) The Medial Temporal Lobe. Annual Reviews Neuroscience, 27, 279- 306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130
  6. Eichenbaum, H., Stewart, C. and Morris, R.G. (1990) Hippocampal Representation in Place Learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 3531-3542.
  7. McNaughton, B.L., Battaglia, F.P., Jensen, O., Moser, E.I. and Moser, M.B. (2006) Path Integration and the Neural Basis of the “Cognitive Map”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 663-678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1932
  8. Morris, R.G. (2006) Theories of Hippocampal Function. In: Andersen, R., Morris, R., Amaral, D., Bliss, T. and O’Keefe, J., Eds., The Hippocampus Book, Oxford University Press, London, 581-712.
  9. O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978) The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
  10. Wiener, J., Shettleworth, S.J., Bingman, V.P., Cheng, K., Healy, S.D., Jacobs, L.F., Jeffery, K.J., Mallot, H.A., Menzel, R. and Newcombe, N.S. (2011) Animal Navigation―A Synthesis. In: Menzel, R. and Fisher, J., Eds., Animal Thinking, MIT Press, Cambridge, 1-33.
  11. Jacobs, L.F. and Menzel, R. (2014) Navigation Outside of the Box: What the Lab Can Learn from the Field and What the Field can Learn from the Lab. Movement Ecology, 2, 3.
  12. Thinus-Blanc, C. (1996) Animal Spatial Cognition: Behavioral and Neural Approaches. World Scientific Publishing Co., Singapore. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/3246
  13. Burgess, N., Jeffery, K.J. and O’Keefe, J. (1999) The Hippocampal and Parietal Foundations of Spatial Cognition. Oxford University Press, London.
  14. Coppola, V.J., Spencer, J.M., Peterson, R.M. and Bingman, V.P. (2014) Hippocampal Lesions in Homing Pigeons Do Not Impair Feature-Quality or Feature-Quantity Discrimination. Behavioural Brain Research, 260, 83-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.038
  15. Fremouw, T., Jackson-Smith, P. and Kesner, R.P. (1997) Impaired Place Learning and Unimpaired Cue Learning in Hippocampal-Lesioned Pigeons. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 963-975. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.111.5.955
  16. Good, M. and Macphail, E.M. (1994) The Avian Hippocampus and Short-Term Memory for Spatial and Non-Spatial Information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B, 47, 293-317.
  17. Hampton, R.R., Hampstead, B.M. and Murray, E.A. (2004) Selective Hippocampal Damage in Rhesus Monkeys Impairs Spatial Memory in an Open-Field Test. Hippocampus, 14, 808-818. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.10217
  18. Sherry, D.F. and Duff, S.J. (1996) Behavioral and Neural Bases of Orientation in Food Storing Birds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 199, 165-172.
  19. Holtzman, D.A., Harris, T.W., Aranguren, G. and Bostock, E. (1999) Spatial Learning of an Escape Task by Young Corn Snakes, Elaphe guttata guttata. Animal Behaviour, 57, 51-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0971
  20. LaDage, L.D., Roth, T.C., Cerjanic, A.M., Sinervo, B. and Pravosudov, V.V. (2012) Spatial Memory: Are Lizards Really Deficient? Biology Letters, 8, 939-941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2012.0527
  21. López, J.C., Rodríguez, F., Gómez, Y., Vargas, J.P., Broglio, C. and Salas, C. (2000) Place and Cue Learning in Turtles. Animal Learning & Behaviour, 28, 360-372. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200270
  22. López, J.C., Gómez, Y., Rodríguez, F., Broglio, C., Vargas, J.P. and Salas, C. (2001) Spatial Learning in Turtles. Animal Cognition, 4, 49-59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710100091
  23. Mueller-Paul, J., Wilkinson, A., Hall, G. and Huber, L. (2012) Radial-Arm-Maze Behaviour of the Red-Footed Tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 305-317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026881
  24. Stone, A., Ford, N.B. and Holtzman, D.A. (2000) Spatial Learning and Shelter Selection by Juvenile Spotted Pythons, Anteresia maculosus. Journal of Herpetology, 34, 575-587. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1565273
  25. Wilkinson, A., Chan, H. and Hall, G, (2007) Spatial Learning and Memory in the Tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 412-418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.412
  26. Wilkinson, A., Coward, S. and Hall, G. (2009) Visual and Response-Based Navigation in the Tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Animal Cognition, 12, 779-787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0237-9
  27. Butler, A.B. and Hodos, H. (2005) Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. Wiley-Liss, New York. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/0471733849
  28. Jarvis, E.D. (2009) Evolution of the Pallium in Birds and Reptiles. In: Binder, M.D., Hirokawa, N. and Windhorst, U. Eds., Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, Springer, Berlin, 1390-1400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_3165
  29. Nieuwenhuys, R., Ten Donkelaar, H.J. and Nicholson, C. (1998) The Central Nervous System of Vertebrates. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-18262-4
  30. Northcutt, R.G. (1981) Evolution of the Telencephalon in Nonmammals. Annual Review Neuroscience, 4, 301-350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.04.030181.001505
  31. Striedter, G.F. (2005) Principles of Brain Evolution. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland.
  32. Ulinski, P.S. (1990) The Cerebral Cortex of Reptiles. In: Jones, E.G. and Peters, A., Eds., Comparative Structure and Evolution of Cerebral Cortex, Part I, Plenum, New York, 139-215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9622-3_5
  33. Schwerdtfeger, W.K. and Smeets, W.J.A.J. (1988) The Forebrain of Reptiles. Current Concepts of Structure and Function. Karger, Basel.
  34. Bruce, L.L. and Butler, A.B. (1984) Telencephalic Connections in Lizards. I. Projections to Cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 229, 585-601. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.902290411
  35. Hoogland, P.V. and Vermeulen-Vanderzee, E. (1993) Medial Cortex of the Lizard (Gecko gecko): A Hodological Study with Emphasis on Regional Specialization. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 331, 326-338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.903310304
  36. López, J.C., Vargas, J.P., Gómez, Y. and Salas, C. (2003) Spatial and Non-Spatial Learning in Turtles: The Role of Medial Cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 143, 109-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00030-5
  37. Rodríguez, F., López, J.C., Vargas, J.P., Gómez, Y., Broglio, C. and Salas, C. (2002) Conservation of Spatial Memory Function in the Pallial Forebrain of Amniotes and Ray Finned-Fishes. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2894-2903.
  38. López, J.C., Gómez, Y., Vargas, J.P. and Salas, C. (2003) Spatial Reversal Learning Deficit after Medial Cortex Lesion in Turtles. Neuroscience Letters, 341, 197-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00186-1
  39. Powers, A.S. (1990) Brain Mechanisms of Learning in Reptiles. In: Kesner, R.P. and Olton, D.S., Eds., Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, 157-177.
  40. Holding, M.L., Frazier, J.A., Taylor, E.N. and Strand, C.R. (2012) Experimentally Altered Navigational Demands Induce Changes in the Cortical Forebrain of Free-Ranging Northern Pacific Rattlesnakes (Crotalus o. oreganus). Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 79, 144-154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000335034
  41. Day, L., Crews, D. and Wilczynski, W. (1999) Relative Medial and Dorsal Cortex Volume in Relation to Foraging Ecology in Congeneric Lizards. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 54, 314-322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000006631
  42. Day, L.B., Crews, D. and Wilczynski, W. (2001) Effects of Medial and Dorsal Cortex Lesions on Spatial Memory in Lizards. Behavioural Brain Research, 118, 27-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00308-9
  43. Roth, E.D., Lutterschmidt, W.I. and Wilson, D.A. (2006) Relative Medial and Dorsal Cortex Volume in Relation to Sex Differences in Spatial Ecology of a Snake Population. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 67, 103-110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000089183
  44. LaDage, L.D., Maged, R.M., Forney, M.V., Roth, T.C., Sinervo, B. and Pravosudov, V.V. (2013) Interaction between Territoriality, Spatial Environment, and Hippocampal Neurogenesis in Male Side-Blotched Lizards. Behavioural Neuroscience, 127, 555-565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032852
  45. Maine, A.R., Powers, S.D. and Lutterschmidt, D.I. (2014) Seasonal Variation in Cell Proliferation and Cell Migration in the Brain of Adult Red-Sided Garter Snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis). Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 84, 181- 196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000364778
  46. Muñoz, M.D., Gaztelu, J.M. and García-Austt, E. (1998) Homo- and Heterosynaptic Long-Term Potentiation in the Medial Cortex of the Turtle Brain in Vitro. Brain Research, 807, 155-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00807-5
  47. Muñoz, M.D., Magariños-Ascone, C., Gaztelu, J.M. and García-Austt, E. (1998) Frequency Potentiation in the Medial Cortex of Young Turtle Brains in Vitro. Brain Behavior & Evolution, 52, 263-269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000006571
  48. Pašukonis, A., Loretto, M.C., Landler, L., Ringler, M. and Hödl, W. (2014) Homing Trajectories and Initial Orientation in a Neotropical Territorial Frog, Allobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae). Frontiers in Zoology, 11, 29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-11-29
  49. Sotelo, M.I., Bingman, V.P. and Muzio, R.N. (2015) Goal Orientation by Geometric and Feature Cues: Spatial Learning in the Terrestrial Toad Rhinella arenarum. Animal Cognition, 18, 315-323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0802-8
  50. Papastamatiou, Y.P., Cartamil, D.P., Lowe, C.G., Meyer, C.G., Wetherbee, B.M. and Holland, K.N. (2011) Scales of Orientation, Directed Walks and Movement Path Structure in Sharks. Journal of Animal Ecology, 80, 864-874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01815.x
  51. Schluessel, V. and Bleckmann, H. (2005) Spatial Memory and Orientation Strategies in the Elasmobranch Potamotrygon motoro. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 191, 695-706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00359-005-0625-9
  52. Schluessel, V. and Bleckmann, H. (2012) Spatial Learning and Memory Retention in the Grey Bamboo Shark (Chiloscyllium griseum). Zoology, 115, 346-353. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.zool.2012.05.001
  53. Nieuwenhuys, R. (1963) The Comparative Anatomy of the Actinopterygian Forebrain. Journal für Hirnforschung, 6, 171-200.
  54. Nieuwenhuys, R. (2011) The Development and General Morphology of the Telencephalon of Actinopterygian Fishes: Synopsis, Documentation and Commentary. Brain Structure & Function, 215, 141-157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0285-6
  55. Northcutt, R.G. and Braford, M.R. (1980) New Observations on the Organization and Evolution of the Telencephalon of Actinopterygian Fishes. In: Ebbesson, S.O.E., Ed., Comparative Neurology of the Telencephalon, Plenum Press, New York, 41-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2988-6_3
  56. Striedter, G.F. and Northcutt, R.G. (2006) Head Size Constrains Forebrain Development and Evolution in Ray-Finned Fishes. Evolution & Development, 8, 215-222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-142X.2006.00091.x
  57. Braford, M.R. (1995) Comparative Aspects of Forebrain Organization in the Ray-Finned Fishes: Touchstones or Not? Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 46, 259-274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000113278
  58. Butler, A.B. (2000) Topography and Topology of the Teleost Telencephalon: A Paradox Resolved. Neuroscience Letters, 293, 95-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01497-X
  59. Nieuwenhuys, R. and Meek, J. (1990) The Telencephalon of Actinopterygian Fishes. In: Jones, E.G. and Peters, A., Eds., Comparative Structure and Evolution of the Cerebral Cortex, Plenum Press, New York, 31-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9622-3_2
  60. Northcutt, R.G. (1995) The Forebrain of Gnathostomes: In Search of a Morphotype. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 46, 275-318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000113279
  61. Northcutt, R.G. (2006) Connections of the Lateral and Medial Divisions of the Goldfish Telencephalic Pallium. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 494, 903-943. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.20853
  62. Kapsimali, M., Vidal, B., Gonzalez, A., Dufour, S. and Vernier, P. (2000) Distribution of the mRNA Encoding the Four Dopamine D(1) Receptor Subtypes in the Brain of the European Eel (Anguilla anguilla): Comparative Approach to the Function of D(1) Receptors in Vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 419, 20-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000410)419:3<320::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-F
  63. Wullimann, M.F. and Mueller, T. (2004) Teleostean and Mammalian Forebrains Contrasted: Evidence from Genes to Behaviour. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 75, 143-162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.20183
  64. Yamamoto, N., Ishikawa, Y., Yoshimoto, M., Xue, H.G., Bahaxar, N., Sawai, N., Yang, C.Y., Ozawa, H. and Ito, H. (2007) A New Interpretation on the Homology of the Teleostean Telencephalon Based on Hodology and a New Eversion Model. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 69, 96-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000095198
  65. Yamamoto, N. and Ito, H. (2008) Visual, Lateral Line, and Auditory Ascending Pathways to the Dorsal Telencephalic Area through the Rostrolateral Region of the Lateral Preglomerular Nucleus in Cyprinids. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 508, 615-647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.21717
  66. Harvey-Girard, E., Giassi, A.C., Ellis, W. and Maler, L. (2012) Organization of the Gymnotiform Fish Pallium in Relation to Learning and Memory: IV. Expression of Conserved Transcription Factors and Implications for the Evolution of Dorsal Telencephalon. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 520, 3395-3413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.23107
  67. Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F. and Salas, C. (2003) Spatial Cognition and Its Neural Basis in Teleost Fishes. Fish & Fisheries, 4, 247-255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00128.x
  68. Durán, E., Ocaña, F.M., Gómez, A., Jiménez-Moya, F., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F. and Salas C. (2008) Telencephalon Ablation Impairs Goldfish Allocentric Spatial Learning in a “Hole-Board” Task. Acta Neurobiologie Experimentalis, 68, 519-525.
  69. López, J.C., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., Thinus-Blanc, C. and Salas C. (1999) Multiple Spatial Learning Strategies in Goldfish (Carassius auratus). Animal Cognition, 2, 109-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s100710050031
  70. Rodríguez, F., Durán, E., Vargas, J.P., Torres, B. and Salas, C. (1994) Performance of Goldfish Trained in Allocentric and Egocentric Maze Procedures Suggests the Presence of a Cognitive Mapping System in Fishes. Animal Learning & Behaviour, 22, 409-420. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209160
  71. Salas, C., Broglio, C., Durán, E., Gómez, A. and Rodríguez, F. (2008) Spatial Learning in Fish. In: Menzel, R., Ed., Learning Theory and Behavior. Vol. 1 of Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, Elsevier, Oxford, 499- 528.
  72. Salas, C., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., López, J.C., Portavella, M. and Torres, B. (1996) Telencephalic Ablation in Goldfish Impairs Performance in a Spatial Constancy Problem but Not in a Cued One. Behavioural Brain Research, 79, 193-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(96)00014-9
  73. Salas, C., Rodríguez, F., Vargas, J.P., Durán, E. and Torres, B. (1996) Spatial Learning and Memory Deficits after Telencephalic Ablation in Goldfish Trained in Place and Turn Maze Procedures. Behavioural Neuroscience, 110, 965- 980. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.110.5.965
  74. Sovrano, V.A., Bisazza, A. and Vallortigara, G. (2003) Modularity as a Fish (Xenotoca eiseni) Views It: Conjoining Geometric and Nongeometric Information for Spatial Reorientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behaviour Processes, 29, 199-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.29.3.199
  75. Vargas, J.P., López, J.C., Salas, C. and Thinus-Blanc, C. (2004) Encoding of Geometric and Featural Spatial Information by Goldfish (Carassius auratus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 206-216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.2.206
  76. Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., Gómez, A., Arias, J.L. and Salas, C. (2010) Selective Involvement of the Goldfish Lateral Pallium in Spatial Memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 210, 191-201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.02.031
  77. Durán, E., Ocaña, F.M., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F. and Salas, C. (2010) Lateral but Not Medial Telencephalic Pallium Ablation Impairs the Use of Goldfish Spatial Allocentric Strategies in a “Hole-Board” Task. Behavioural Brain Research, 214, 480-487. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.010
  78. Hampton, R.R. and Shettleworth, S.J. (1996) Hippocampal Lesions Impair Memory for Location but Not Color in Passerine Birds. Behavioural Neuroscience, 110, 831-835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.110.4.831
  79. Vargas, J.P., Rodríguez, F., López, J.C., Arias, J.L. and Salas, C. (2000) Spatial Learning-Induced Increase in the Argyrophilic Nucleolar Organizer Region of Dorsolateral Telencephalic Neurons in Goldfish. Brain Research, 865, 77-84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02220-4
  80. Derenzini, M. (2000) The AgNORs. Micron, 31, 117-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0968-4328(99)00067-0
  81. Conejo, N.M., González-Pardo, H., Gonzalez-Lima, F. and Arias, J.L. (2010) Spatial Learning of the Water Maze: Progression of Brain Circuits Mapped with Cytochrome Oxidase Histochemistry. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 93, 362-371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2009.12.002
  82. Gonzalez-Lima, F. and Cada, A. (1994) Cytochrome Oxidase Activity in the Auditory System of the Mouse: A Qualitative and Quantitative Histochemical Study. Neuroscience, 63, 559-578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(94)90550-9
  83. Poremba, A., Jones, D. and Gonzalez-Lima, F. (1998) Classical Conditioning Modifies Cytochrome Oxidase Activity in the Auditory System. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 3035-3043. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00304.x
  84. Wong-Riley, M.T. (1989) Cytochrome Oxidase: An Endogenous Metabolic Marker for Neuronal Activity. Trends in Neurosciences, 12, 94-101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(89)90165-3
  85. Uceda, S., Ocaña, F.M., Martín-Monzón, I., Rodríguez-Expósito, B., Broglio, C., Durán, E. and Rodríguez, F. (2015) Spatial Learning-Related Changes in Metabolic Brain Activity Contributes to the Delimitation of the Hippocampal Pallium in Goldfish. In Preparation.
  86. Costa, S.S., Andrade, R., Carneiro, L.A., Goncalves, E.J., Kotrschal, K. and Oliveira, R.F. (2011) Sex Differences in the Dorsolateral Telencephalon Correlate with Home Range Size in Blenniid Fish. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 77, 55-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000323668
  87. Shumway, C.A. (2008) Habitat Complexity, Brain, and Behaviour. Brain Behaviour & Evolution, 72, 123-134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000151472
  88. Bachy, I., Berthon, J. and Rétaux, S. (2002) Defining Pallial and Subpallial Compartments in the Developing Xenopus Forebrain. Mechanisms of Development, 117, 163-172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0925-4773(02)00199-5
  89. Garda, A.L., Puelles, L., Rubenstein, J.L.R. and Medina, L. (2002) Expression Patterns of Wnt8b and Wnt7b in the Chicken Embryonic Brain Suggests a Correlation with Forebrain Organizers. Neuroscience, 113, 689-698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00171-9
  90. Medina, L., Brox, A., Legaz, I., García-López, M. and Puelles, L. (2005) Expression Patterns of Developmental Regulatory Genes Show Comparable Divisions in the Telencephalon of Xenopus and Mouse: Insights into the Evolution of the Forebrain. Brain Research Bulletin, 66, 297-302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.02.003
  91. Puelles, L. (2001) Thoughts on the Development, Structure and Evolution of the Mammalian and Avian Telencephalic Pallium. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 356, 1583-1598. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2001.0973
  92. Smith-Fernandez, A., Pieau, C., Reperant, J., Boncinelli, E. and Wassef, M. (1998) Expression of the Emx-1 and Dlx-1 Homeobox Genes Define Three Molecularly Distinct Domains in the Telencephalon of Mouse, Chick, Turtle and Frog Embryos: Implications for the Evolution of Telencephalic Subdivisions in Amniotes. Development, 125, 2099-2111.

NOTES

*Corresponding author.