A. HADI
“Human communication rests on a tension between the goals of
communicators and audiences”. In fact, he believes that we, as
communicators, interact to fulfill our benefit and interest, but it
does not mean that we always tell the truth.
Sarangi and Slembrouck (1992) also criticized the Gricean
claim for the normality of cooperation. They applied a Gricean
pragmatic approach to institutional discourse and suggested that
Grice’s framework should be extended to include societal fac-
tors such as the social position of the communicators. They said,
“If we are to follow the Gricean notion of cooperation, the in-
stitution, in such circumstances, would be expected to adopt, at
least from the client’s point of view the client’s goal as its own,
or act towards negotiating a ‘mutually accepted goal’.”
Thus many researchers have questioned or rejected the uni-
versality as well as the feasibility of Grice’s cooperative princi-
ple. Grice’s theory is too biased towards the notion of coopera-
tion in human conversation. But he cannot answer questions
about what would happen in situations where human beings
prefer employing non-cooperative strategies; or how the coop-
erative principle accounts for miscommunication.
Comments on Grice’s Theory
In his article, Ladegaard (2008) suggests that both the se-
mantic and the pragmatic sides of human interaction as well as
all the linguistic awareness necessary for the perception and
interpretation of meaning in any communicative behavior should
be covered in any theor y of con versational coopera tio n.
He argues that Grice only considers the semantic aspect of an
utterance and then makes it clear based on pragmatics, or ac-
cording to the context which help us to interpret the speaker’s
intentions.
Gumperz (1982) argues that it is necessary for the speakers
to take into account all the contextual clues which exist in va-
rious discourse types. These include turn-taking strategies, spe-
ech accommodation, and voice alterations. Ladegaard (2008)
also adds that in order to understand the intention of speaker
accurately in an interaction, and interpret the underlying mean-
ing of an utterance, the use of these cues is really essential.
Ladegaard (2008) states that instead of applying the tradi-
tional view to language and communication offered in Prag-
matics, in which human interaction is viewed as naturally defi-
cient and problematic, a broader view should be considered. He
mentions that Grice is extremely biased towards cooperation.
Grice’s assumption is that people communicate logically, and
all of them attempt to be “good” communicators.
However, Ladegaard’s (2008) analysis conflicts with Grice’s
position. He claims “human interaction may be irrational and
illogical, and that resistance and non cooperation may be adop-
ted as the preferred discursive strategy, and that interactants
seem to try their best to be ‘bad’ communicators.”
In his study Ladegaard (2008), considered the two types of
cooperation related to a Gricean theory: “social goal-sharing
and linguistic goal-sharing”. In this analysis, teachers interview
students regarding their future career. The aim is investigating
attitude-behaviour relationships in language.
Ladegaard’s results show that students’ dialogues are non-
cooperative and non-accommodative, and that these are the
preferred discourse strategies used by students. In other words,
in their interviews students, try to miscommunicate rather than
to communicate successfully. Ladegaard’s believes social and
psychological conditions determine people’s intensions as to
whether or not to cooperate in a conversation.
Conclusion
In summary, Grice’s theory is flawed. First, it is too biased
towards cooperation. Grice believes that people aims at com-
municating successfully and effectively and in trying to solve
their problems. Actually, he neglected the fact that there are
times when the purpose is to intentionally miscommunicate.
Second, his theory is fundamentally asocial. We can say that
he follows Chomsky’s idea (1965: p. 4), of positing an “ideal
speaker-listener in a completely homogeneous speech commu-
nity.” Therefore, he fails to explain how people actually com-
municate concerning sophisticated social contexts, for instance
if speakers aim to be accepted in all social settings in which
they find themselves.
Since Grice’s theory does not take the social contexts into
account, and only considers the speaker-listener interaction in
an ideal context, and applies universally (regardless of social
elements such as sex, power relationships, social class, and age)
it has little explanatory power.
Based on the Cooperative Principle, people are naturally di-
rected towards cooperation. In other words, they often want
their interactions to succeed, they want to solve problems and
discuss solutions. But, sometimes the purpose is to fail and
undermine the conversation, and to be sure that one does not
achieve his goal, i.e. to prefer to miscommunicate. As Mey
(2001) claims, in flouting a maxim, people are trying to be
non-cooperative to indicate their resistance, so the effects they
are aiming for should be considered cautiously.
As stated above, Grice is interested in finding the logic of
conversation and how we can explain the gap between saying
and meaning, saying and implicating, conventional and non-
conventional meaning. This logic, in his view, is considered as
a manifestation of rational acts.
His theory is inflexible, because it does not consider the fact
that human communication like his nature is a complicated,
diverse and rich phenomenon. Moreover, it disregards the
situations where the interactants’s goal is to miscommunicate.
As Sarangi and Slemb r o u c h (1992: p. 142) assert:
“A sufficient theory should illustrate how people get the
speakers’ intention by pointing out the social positioning
of the language users and the societal bearings on the
situational context.”
In conclusion, although being based on introspection rather
than data, and does not consider interpersonal factors, Grice’s
work faces major limitations, it is still at the centre of the disci-
plines of pragmatics and the important role it plays in this field
cannot be denied. However we should be careful interpreting
what is meant by “cooperation” in Grice’s CP. His notion is
different from the everyday notion of cooperation. Some au-
thors make this difference clear to readers. To have a fair un-
derstanding of the Grice’ CP, it would be better to study it in
the context of Grice’s works as a whole rather than in isolation.
REFERENCES
Brown, G., & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511805226
Davies, B. L. (2008). Grice’s cooperative principle: Meaning and ra-
tionality. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 2308-2331.
Copyright © 2013 SciRes. 71