Human Mirror Neuron System (hMNS) Specific Differences in Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Self-Reported Democrats and Republicans: A Pilot Study

Abstract

In light of ever-present partisan division in the US political system, it is critical that researchers gain a better under-standing of potential biological differences that exist between self-professed Democrats and Republicans. In the current pilot experiment, we examined differences within the human mirror neuron system (hMNS), a network linked to a host of social and emotional abilities, in a small group of self-identified Republicans and Democrats. We found clear differences between these two groups with respect to resting-state brain connectivity within the hMNS. These neural differences were not systematically related to differences in empathy. Our findings are consistent with the idea that other factors, such as one’s preferential type of social connectivity (broad vs. tight), may have driven the reported findings. These data provide novel insights regarding our knowledge of the biological basis of party identification, and suggest specific directions for future research.

Share and Cite:

R. Newman-Norlund, J. Burch and K. Becofsky, "Human Mirror Neuron System (hMNS) Specific Differences in Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Self-Reported Democrats and Republicans: A Pilot Study," Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science, Vol. 3 No. 4, 2013, pp. 341-349. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2013.34034.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] K. E. Poole and H. Rosenthal, “Congress: A PoliticalEconomic History of Roll Call Voting,” Oxford University Press, New York, 1997.
[2] A. J. Hoffman, “The Growing Climate Divide,” Sociology, 1, 2011, pp. 195-196.
[3] J. M. Stonecash, M. D. Brewer and M. D. Mariani, “Diverging Parties: Realignment, Social Change, and Political Polarization,” Westview Press, Boulder, 2002.
[4] N. McCarty, K. T. Poole and H. Rosenthal, “Polarized America: The Dance of Ideology and Unequal Riches,” MIT Press, Cambridge, 2008.
[5] G. C. Layman, T. M. Carsey and J. M. Horowicz, “Party Polarization in American Politics: Characteristics, Causes and Consequences,” Annual Review of Political Science, Vol. 9, 2006, pp. 83-110. doi:10.1146/annurev.polisci.9.070204.105138
[6] J. T. Kaplan, J. Freedman and M. Iacoboni, “Us versus Them: Political Attitudes and Party Affiliation Influence Neural Response to Faces of Presidential Candidates,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 45, No. 1, 2007, pp. 55-64. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.024
[7] J. T. Jost and D. M. Amodio, “Political Ideology as Motivated Social Cognition: Behavioral and Neuroscientific Evidence,” Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 36, No. 1, 2012, pp. 55-64. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7
[8] J. T. Jost, J. L. Napier, H. Thorisdottir, S. D. Gosling, T. P. Palfai and B. Ostafin, “Are Needs to Manage Uncertainty and Threat Associated with Political Conservatism or Ideological Extremity?” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 33, No. 7, 2007, pp. 989-1007. doi:10.1177/0146167207301028
[9] D. M. Amodio, J. T. Jost, S. L. Master and C. M. Yee, “Neurocognitive Correlates of Liberalism and Conservatism,” Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 10, 2007, pp. 1246-1247. doi:10.1038/nn1979
[10] D. M. Amodio, S. L. Master, C. M. Yee and S. E. Taylor, “Neurocognitive Components of Behavioal Inhibition and Activation Systems: Implications for Theories of SelfRegulation,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 45, 2008, pp. 11-19.
[11] G. Zamboni, et al., “Individualism, Conservatism, and Radicalism as Criteria for Processing Political Beliefs: A Parametric fMRI Study,” Society for Neuroscience, Vol. 4, No. 5, 2009, pp. 367-383. doi:10.1080/17470910902860308
[12] R. Kanai, et al., “Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults,” Current Biology, Vol. 21, No. 8, 2011, pp. 677-680. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017
[13] J. H. Fowler and D. Schreiber, “Biology, Politics and the Emerging Science of Human Nature,” Science, Vol. 322, No. 5903, 2008, pp. 912-914. doi:10.1126/science.1158188
[14] P. K. Hatemi and R. McDermott, “Broadening Political Psychology,” Political Psychology, Vol. 33, No. 1, 2012, pp. 11-25. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00867.x
[15] M. J. Weissflog, S. J. R. van Noordt, B. L. Choma, J. Dywan and S. J. Segalowitz, “Sociopolitical Ideology and Electrocortical Responses,” Psychophysiology, Vol. 47, 2010, p. 24.
[16] M. H. Davis, “Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 44, No. 1, 1983, pp. 113-126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
[17] C. P. McCue and J. D. Gopoian, “Dispositional Empathy and the Political Gender Gap,” Women and Politics, Vol. 21, No. 2, 2008, pp. 1-20. doi:10.1300/J014v21n02_01
[18] A. S. Gerber, G. A. Huber, D. Doherty and C. M. Dowling, “Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan Identification,” Political Behavior, Vol. 34, No. 4, 2011, pp. 653-688.
[19] R. Iyer, J. Graham, S. Koleva, P. Ditto and J. Haidt, “Beyond Identity Politics: Moral Psychology and the 2008 Democratic Primary,” Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, Vol. 10, No. 1, 2010, pp. 293-306.
[20] J. J. Mondak and K. D. Halperin, “A Framework for the Study of Personality and Political Behaviour,” British Journal of Political Science, Vol. 38, No. 2, 2008, pp. 335-362. doi:10.1017/S0007123408000173
[21] R. Iyer, S. Koleva, J. Graham, P. Ditto and J. Haidt, “Understanding Libertarian Morality: The Psychological Dispositions of Self-Identified Libertarians,” PLoS One, Vol. 7, No. 8, 2012, Article ID: e42366. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042366
[22] G. di Pellegrino, et al., “Understanding Motor Events: A Neurophysiological Study,” Experimental Brain Research, Vol. 91, No. 1, 1992, pp. 176-180. doi:10.1007/BF00230027
[23] N. Sebanz and C. Frith, “Beyond Simulation? Neural Mechanisms for Predicting the Actions of others,” Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 7, No. 1, 2004, pp. 5-6. doi:10.1038/nn0104-5
[24] M. Iacoboni, et al., “Grasping the Intentions of Others with One’s Own Mirror Neuron System,” PLoS Biology, Vol. 3, No. 3, 2005, Article ID: e79. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079
[25] G. Rizzolatti and L. Craighero, “The Mirror-Neuron System,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol. 27, 2004, pp. 169-192. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
[26] V. Gallese and A. Goldman, “Mirror Neurons and the Simulation Theory of Mind-Reading,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 2, No. 12, 1998, pp. 493-501. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5
[27] H. Theoret, et al., “Impaired Motor Facilitation during Action Observation in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder,” Current Biology, Vol. 15, No. 3, 2005, pp. R84-R85. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.022
[28] L. Q. Uddin, et al., “Self-Face Recognition Activates a Frontoparietal ‘Mirror’ Network in the Right Hemisphere: An Event-Related fMRI Study,” Neuroimage, Vol. 25, No. 3, 2005, pp. 926-935. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.018
[29] S. Atmaca, et al., “Action Co-Representation: The Joint SNARC Effect,” Society for Neuroscience, Vol. 3, No. 3-4, 2008, pp. 410-420.
[30] L. Aziz-Zadeh, et al., “Lateralization of the Human Mirror Neuron System,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 26, No. 11, 2006, pp. 2964-2970. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2921-05.2006
[31] I. Molnar-Szakacs, et al., “Observing Complex Action Sequences: The Role of the Fronto-Parietal Mirror Neuron System,” Neuroimage, Vol. 33, No. 3, 2006, pp. 923-935. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.035
[32] J. M. Kilner, et al., “Evidence of Mirror Neurons in Human Inferior Frontal Gyrus,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 29, No. 32, 2009, pp. 10153-10159. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009
[33] A. F. Hamilton and S. T. Grafton, “Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus,” Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 26, No. 4, 2006, pp. 1133-1137. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006
[34] C. Catmur, V. Walsh and C. Heyes, “Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System,” Current Biology, Vol. 17, No. 17, 2007, pp. 1527-1531.
[35] L. Fadiga, et al., “Motor Facilitation during Action Observation: A Magnetic Stimulation Study,” Journal of Neurophysiology, Vol. 73, No. 6, 1995, pp. 2608-2611.
[36] R. D. Newman-Norlund, et al., “Virtual Lesions of the IFG Abolish Response Facilitation for Biological and Non-Biological Cues,” Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 4, 2010, p. 5.
[37] M. Heiser, et al., “The Essential Role of Broca’s Area in Imitation,” European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 17, No. 5, 2003, pp. 1123-1128. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02530.x
[38] R. Mukamel, et al., “Single-Neuron Responses in Humans during Execution and Observation of Actions,” Current Biology, Vol. 20, No. 8, 2010, pp. 750-756. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045
[39] L. Cattaneo, et al., “Impairment of Actions Chains in Autism and Its Possible Role in Intention Understanding,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 104, No. 45, 2007, pp. 17825-17530. doi:10.1073/pnas.0706273104
[40] J. C. Carmo, R. I. Rumiati and A. Vallesi, “Understanding and Imitating Unfamiliar Actions: Distinct Underlying Mechanisms,” PLoS One, Vol. 7, No. 10, 2012, Article ID: e46939. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046939
[41] L. Fogassi, et al., “Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding,” Science, Vol. 308, No. 5722, 2005, pp. 662-667. doi:10.1126/science.1106138
[42] V. Gallese, “Embodied Simulation: From Mirror Neuron Systems to Interpersonal Relations,” Novartis Foundation Symposium, Vol. 278, 2007, pp. 3-12.
[43] L. M. Oberman, and V. S. Ramachandran, “The Simulating Social Mind: The Role of the Mirror Neuron System and Simulation in the Social and Communicative Deficits of Autism Spectrum Disorders,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 133, No. 2, 2007, pp. 310-327. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.310
[44] J. M. Kilner, K. J. Friston and C. D. Frith, “Predictive Coding: An Account of the Mirror Neuron System,” Cognitive Processing, Vol. 8, No. 3, 2007, pp. 159-166. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2
[45] F. P. de Lange, et al., “Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions,” Current Biology, Vol. 18, No. 6, 2008, pp. 454-457. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.057
[46] G. Buccino, et al., “The Neural Basis for Understanding Non-Intended Actions,” Neuroimage, Vol. 36, Suppl. 2, 2007, pp. T119-T127. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.036
[47] N. Canessa, et al., “The Neural Bases of Social Intention Understanding: The Role of Interaction Goals,” PLoS One, Vol. 7, No. 7, Article ID: e42347. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042347
[48] M. Schulte-Ruther, et al., “Gender Differences in Brain Networks Supporting Empathy,” Neuroimage, Vol. 42, No. 1, 2008, pp. 393-403. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.180
[49] L. F. Hausser, “Empathy and Mirror Neurons. A View on Contemporary Neuropsychological Empathy Research,” Praxis der Kinderpsychologie and Kinderpsychiatrie, Vol. 61, No. 5, 2012, pp. 322-335.
[50] J. T. Kaplan and M. Iacoboni, “Getting a Grip on Other Minds: Mirror Neurons, Intention Understanding, and Cognitive Empathy,” Social Neuroscience, Vol. 1, No. 3-4, 2006, pp. 175-183. doi:10.1080/17470910600985605
[51] A. D. Baird, I. E. Scheffer and S. J. Wilson, “Mirror Neuron System Involvement in Empathy: A Critical Look at the Evidence,” Social Neuroscience, Vol. 6, No. 4, 2011, pp. 327-335. doi:10.1080/17470919.2010.547085
[52] S. A. Morelli, L. T. Rameson and M. D. Lieberman, “The Neural Components of Empathy: Predicting Daily Prosocial Behavior,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2012.
[53] J. H. Pfeifer, et al., “Mirroring Others’ Emotions Relates to Empathy and Interpersonal Competence in Children,” Neuroimage, Vol. 39, No. 4, 2008, pp. 2076-2085. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.032
[54] S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, J. Aharon-Peretz and D. Perry, “Two Systems for Empathy: A Double Dissociation between Emotional and Cognitive Empathy in Inferior Frontal Gyrus versus Ventromedial Prefrontal Lesions,” Brain, Vol. 132, No. 3, 2009, pp. 617-627. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279
[55] V. Gallese, M. N. Eagle and P. Migone, “Intentional Attunement: Mirror Neurons and the Neural Underpinnings of Interpersonal Relations,” Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Vol. 55, No. 1, 2007, pp. 131-176. doi:10.1177/00030651070550010601
[56] F. Chersi, “Neural Mechanisms and Models Underlying Joint Action,” Experimental Brain Research, Vol. 211, No. 3-4, 2011, pp. 643-653. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2690-3
[57] S. Ortigue, et al., “Understanding Actions of Others: The Electrodynamics of the Left and Right Hemispheres. A High-Density EEG Neuroimaging Study,” PLoS One, Vol. 5, No. 8, 2010, Article ID: e12160. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012160
[58] A. M. B. Stoit, H. T. van Schie, M. Riem, R. G. J. Meu-lenbroek, R. D. Newman-Norlund, H. Bekkering and B J. K. Uitelaar, “Internal Model Deficits Impair Joint Action in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders,” Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Vol. 5, No. 4, 2011, pp. 1526-1537. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.02.016
[59] R. D. Newman-Norlund, et al., “Anatomical Substrates of Cooperative Joint-Action in a Continuous Motor Task: Virtual Lifting and Balancing,” Neuroimage, Vol. 41, No. 1, 2008, pp. 169-177. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.026
[60] R. D. Newman-Norlund, et al., “The Mirror Neuron System Is More Active during Complementary Compared with Imitative Action,” Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 10, No. 7, 2007, pp. 817-818.
[61] L. Dossey, “Strange Contagions: Of Laughter, Jumps, Jerks, and Mirror Neurons,” EXPLORE: The Journal of Science and Healing, Vol. 6, No. 3, 2010, pp. 119-128. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2010.03.001
[62] C. A. Bouquet, et al., “Motor Contagion: Goal-Directed Actions Are More Contagious Than Non-Goal-Directed Actions,” Experimental Psychology, Vol. 58, No. 1, 2011, pp. 71-78. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000069
[63] N. R. Cooper, et al., “Bridging a Yawning Chasm: EEG Investigations into the Debate Concerning the Role of the Human Mirror Neuron System in Contagious Yawning,” Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 12, No. 2, 2012, pp. 393-405.doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0081-7
[64] H. Haker, et al., “Mirror Neuron Activity during Contagious Yawning—An fMRI Study,” Brain Imaging and Behavior, Vol. 7, No. 1, 2013, pp. 28-34.
[65] K. M. Knutson, et al., “Politics on the Brain: An FMRI Investigation,” Social Neuroscience, Vol. 1, No. 1, 2006, pp. 25-40. doi:10.1080/17470910600670603
[66] M. P. Van den Heuvel and H. E. H. Pol, “Exploring the Brain Network: A Review on Resting-State fMRI Functional Connectivity,” European Neuropsychopharmacology, Vol. 20, No. 8, 2010, pp. 519-534. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008
[67] J. S. Adelstein, et al., “Personality Is Reflected in the Brain’s Intrinsic Functional Architecture,” PLoS One, Vol. 6, No. 11, 2011, Article ID: e27633. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027633
[68] R. Sala-Llonch, et al., “Brain Connectivity during Resting State and Subsequent Working Memory Task Predicts Behavioural Performance,” Cortex, Vol. 48, No. 9, 2011, pp. 1187-1196. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.07.006
[69] A. Martin, K. A. Barnes and W. D. Stevens, “Spontaneous Neural Activity Predicts Individual Differences in Performance,” Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 109, No. 9, 2012, pp. 3201-3202. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200329109
[70] E. A. von dem Hagen, et al., “Reduced Functional Connectivity within and between ‘Social’ Resting State Networks in Autism Spectrum Conditions,” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2012.
[71] A. Baldassarre, et al., “Individual Variability in Functional Connectivity Predicts Performance of a Perceptual Task,” Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 109, No. 9, 2012, pp. 3516-3521. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113148109
[72] D. Wendt, “Political Ideology Questionnaire,” 2012. https://wveis.k12.wv.us/teach21/cso/upload/LP1509WS1.doc
[73] T. M. Singelis, H. C. Triandis, D. P. S. Bhawuk and M. J. Gelfland, “Horizontal and Vertical Dimensions of Individualism and Collectivism: A Theoretical and Measurement Refinement,” Cross-Cultural Research, Vol. 29, No. 3, 1995, pp. 240-275. doi:10.1177/106939719502900302
[74] S. McFarland and D. Brown, “Who Believes that Identification with all Humanity Is Ethical?” Psicologia Politica, Vol. 36, 2008, pp. 37-49.
[75] B. Campos, D. Keltner and G. C. Gonzaga, “Different Kinds of Love: How Love Experiences Differ across Relationships in Western Psychological Association,” 2002.
[76] M. Tsakiris, M. R. Longo and P. Haggard, “Having a Body versus Moving Your Body: Neural Signatures of Agency and Body-Ownership,” Neuropsychologia, Vol. 48, No. 9, 2010, pp. 2740-2749. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.021
[77] C. Farrer, et al., “Effect of Distorted Visual Feedback on the Sense of Agency,” Behavioral Neurology, Vol. 1, No. 1-2, 2008, pp. 53-57.
[78] L. V. der Heiden, S. Scherpiet, L. Konicar, N. Birbaumer and R. Veit, “Inter-Individual Differences in Successful Perspective Taking during Pain Perception Mediates Emotional Responsiveness in Self and Others: An fMRI Study,” Neuroimage, Vol. 65, 2012, pp. 387-394.
[79] D. De Ridder, et al., “Visualizing Out-of-Body Experience in the Brain,” The New England Journal of Medicine, Vol. 357, No. 18, 2007, pp. 1829-1833. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa070010
[80] J. E. Settle, C. T. Dawes and J. H. Fowler, “The Heritability of Partisan Attachment,” Political Research Quarterly, Vol. 62, No. 3, 2009, pp. 601-613. doi:10.1177/1065912908327607
[81] P. Lichtenstein, et al., “Common Genetic Determinants of Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder in Swedish Families: A Population-Based Study,” The Lancet, Vol. 373, No. 9659, 2009, pp. 234-249. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60072-6
[82] U. M. Mehta, et al., “Mirror Neuron Dysfunction—A Neuro-Marker for Social Cognition Deficits in Drug Naive Schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, Vol. 141, No. 2-3, 2012, pp. 281-283. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.07.005
[83] P. G. Enticott, et al., “Reduced Motor Facilitation during Action Observation in Schizophrenia: A Mirror Neuron Deficit?” Schizophrenia Research, Vol. 102, No. 1-3, 2008, pp. 116-121. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.04.001
[84] J. Hallmayer, et al., “Genetic Heritability and Shared Environmental Factors among Twin Pairs with Autism,” Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 68, No. 11, 2011, pp. 1095-1102. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76
[85] M. Dapretto, et al., “Understanding Emotions in Others: Mirror Neuron Dysfunction in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders,” Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 9, No. 1, 2006, pp. 28-30.
[86] N. Hadjikhani et al., “Anatomical Differences in the Mirror Neuron System and Social Cognition Network in Autism,” Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 16, No. 9, 2006, pp. 1276-1282. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj069
[87] R. G. Niemi and M. K. Jennings, “Issues and Inheritance in the Formation of Party Identification,” Journal of Political Science, Vol. 35, No. 4, 1991, pp. 970-988.
[88] M. Jabbi, J. Bastiaansen and C. Keysers, “A Common Anterior Insula Representation of Disgust Observation, Experience and Imagination Shows Divergent Functional Connectivity Pathways,” PLoS One, Vol. 3, No. 8, 2008, Article ID: e2939. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002939
[89] J. A. Pineda, “Sensorimotor Cortex as a Critical Component of an ‘Extended’ Mirror Neuron System: Does it Solve the Development, Correspondence, and Control Problems in Mirroring?” Behavioral and Brain Functions, Vol. 4, 2008, p. 47. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-4-47
[90] M. P. Lopez-Larson, J. S. Anderson, M. A. Ferguson and D. Yurgelun-Todd, “Local Brain Connectivity and Associations with Gender and Age,” Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 1, No. 2, 2011, pp. 187-197. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.10.001

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.