From physical mobility to psychological mobilization: The benefits of changing location and caregiver in a mobile team for adolescent psychiatry

Abstract

We have developed a mobile team for adolescents with psychological difficulties that can intervene within 48 hours, if necessary. Practically, caregivers working in pairs go wherever the adolescents are (home, General Practioner’s office, etc.), travelling in a motorhome designed as a mobile office. This method allows us to approach more closely the adolescents and their families in the here and now that corresponds to our societal evolution. Most of the adolescents seen by the mobile team do not express any explicit demand but the demand is expressed by the professionals in direct contact with these adolescents in difficulty (school professionals, General Practitioners, etc.), who alert the mobile team and solicit the adolescents’ parents to call the mobile team. One characteristic of this mobile team is the mobility of setting (by changing locations, including the use of a mobile office, and caregivers, so that multiple representations are mobilized). Statistical analyses conducted on 520 adolescents followed by the mobile team showed that the first meetings tend to take place in the family’s home, subsequently transferring to the mobile office and finally in the community mental health centres. There is therefore a transition from the living space (a place that is part of the here and now) to the caregiving space (a space of thought and for thinking), with the mobile office serving as an intermediate step in this movement towards accessing care. Furthermore, there was a significant association between changes of locations and continuity of care. Thus, there were significantly more breakdowns in continuity when the consultations all take place in a single location (family home, mobile office, community centre). The results and their therapeutic implications will be discussed in this article, especially with regard to the key role played by physical movement in psychological mobilization, and by plurality of representations associated with different locations and caregivers.

Share and Cite:

Tordjman, S. (2013) From physical mobility to psychological mobilization: The benefits of changing location and caregiver in a mobile team for adolescent psychiatry. Health, 5, 33-40. doi: 10.4236/health.2013.56A3006.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Becker, J. (2006) Relation of neurological findings on decoupling of brain activity from limb movement to Piagetian ideas on the origin of thought. Cognitive Development, 21, 194-198. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.11.002
[2] Piaget, J. (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. Norton (original work published 1936), New York. doi:10.1037/11494-000
[3] Piaget, J. (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books (original work published 1937), New York. doi:10.1037/11168-000
[4] Piaget, J. (1985) The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development. University of Chicago Press (original work published 1975), Chicago.
[5] Carmena, J.M., Carmena, M.A., Lebedev, M.A., Crist, R.E., O’Doherty, J.E., Santucci, D.M., Dimitrov, D.F., Patil, P.G., Henriquez, C.S. and Nicolelis, M.A. (2003) Learning to control a brain-machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. Public Library of Science Biology, 1, 193-208.
[6] Raab, M. and Green, N. (2005) Motion as input: A functional explanation of movement effects on cognitive processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, 333-348. doi:10.2466/pms.100.2.333-348
[7] Berthoz, A. (2003) Le sens du mouvement. Odile Jacob, Paris.
[8] Jellinek, E.H. (2000) Dr. HC Bastian, scientific Jekyll and Hyde. The Lancet, 356, 2180-2183. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03508-X
[9] Rousseau, J.J. (1969) L’emile. NRF Gallimard, Paris.
[10] Bullinger, A. (1991) Le concept d’instrumentation: Son intérêt pour l’approche des différents déficits. In: Deleau, M., Ed., Collection Croissance de l’Enfant, Génèse de l’Homme, Presses Universitaires de France, Paris.
[11] Bachelet, T. (1876) Dictionnaire général des lettres, des beaux-arts et des sciences morales et politiques. Delagrave, Paris.
[12] Rizzolatti, G. and Arbib, M.A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188-194. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01260-0
[13] Rizzolatti, G., Gartilucci, M., Camarda, R.M., Gallex, V., Luppino, G., Matelli, M. and Fogassi, L. (1990) Neurons related to reaching-grasping arm movements in the rostral part of area 6 (area 6a). Experimental Brain Research, 82, 337-350. doi:10.1007/BF00231253
[14] Decety, J., Jeannerod, M. and Prablanc, C. (1989) The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioral Brain Research, 34, 35-42. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(89)80088-9
[15] Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M. and Pastène, J. (1991) Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort. Behavioral Brain Research, 24, 1-5. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80033-6
[16] Mauss, M. (2007) Essai sur le don. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.