Chinese Studies, 2015, 4, 116-126
Published Online November 2015 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/chnstd
http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/chnstd.2015.44017
How to cite this paper: Cai Jie, & Luo Jing (2015). An Empirical Study on Different Tendencies toward Indulgence-Restraint
Dimension from the Intercultural Perspective. Chinese Studies, 4, 116-126. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/chnstd.2015.44017
An Empirical Study on Different Tendencies
toward Indulgence-Restraint Dimension
from the Intercultural Perspective
In the Case of Guangdong Province
Cai Jie, Luo Jing*
Shenzhen Tourism College, Jinan University, Shenzhen, China
Received 14 August 2015; accepted 13 November 2015; published 16 November 2015
Copyright © 2015 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY).
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Abstract
With the booming development of economic globalization nowadays, intercultural communication
becomes more and more important. Based on Hofstede’s indulgence-restraint dimension, this the-
sis aims to testify whether Hofstede’s opinion that China is largely a restrained country, is correct
in Guangdong Province, a coastal area with the largest population of China, and to investigate dif-
ferent groups of people’s different tendencies toward indulgence and restraint. The study is car-
ried out in Guangdong Province, because Guangdong is a special area which is one of the most ra-
pidly developing areas in China and it locates next to Macau and Hong Kong, which may bring it
with more different cultures and ideas. After conducting a questionnaire survey in Guangdong
with a self-made questionnaire, the author gets the following findings. 1) The subjects generally
show a stronger tendency toward indulgence than to restraint through comparing the mean
scores of them respectively. 2) With respect to genders, males have fewer tendencies toward in-
dulgence than females and more tendencies toward restraint. 3) Considering age groups, people
aged more have more tendencies toward restraint and fewer tendencies toward indulgence.
What’s more, people aged from 18 to 60 are generally indulgence-oriented while people aged
above 60 years old are generally restraint-oriented. 4) People with different disposable income
groups and different educational backgrounds have no significant difference in general. All of
them, generally, tend to be indulgent. The results of the study can help both foreign and local
people to know more about Guangdong so as to enhance the mutual understanding to improve
communication efficiency and help construct a harmonious society.
Keywords
Intercultural Communication, Indulgence, Restraint, Guangdong Province
*Corresponding a uthor.
Cai J., Luo J.
117
1. Introduction
With the booming development of global economy nowadays, people travel around the world to do business, to
have fun, to study, etc. However, due to the different cultures, people may find intercultural communication dif-
ficult.
Thus, many scholars have focused a lot on the study of intercultural communication. Intercultural communi-
cation study originated from the USA. As early as in 1959, Edward T. Hall published Silent Language, the fa m-
ous book, which is usually regarded as the foundational work in the cross-cultural field, marking th e beginning
of intercultural study.
Hofstede, a Dutch management scholar, initially carried out a survey about cultural value among around
110,000 employees from 40 nation s in IBM at the end of 1970s. Afte r a detailed analysis o f the data by statistic-
al method, Geert Hofstede (1980) originally identified four cultural dimensions: 1) power distance; 2) uncer-
tainty avoidance; 3) indiv idualism-collectivism; 4) masculinity-femininity. And Hofstede’s study kept going. In
1987, he put forward the fifth dimension, which is “long-term/short-term orientation”. In 2010, the sixth and the
latest dimension were carried out, the indulgence-restraint dimension.
In the theoretical framework of Hofstede, the present study mainly aims to testify if Guangdong Province with
its fast economic development, special policies and location, is largely a restraint-oriented area as Hofstde de-
fined China to be. What’s more, this study also aims to explore the different tendencies toward IVR by different
groups of people at different layers.
The significance of the study lies as below:
Firstly, the thesis can, to some extends, change the foreigners’ stereotype toward people in Guangdong and
help them have a full understanding of Guangdong cultural value to promote the mutual understanding of people
from differe nt c ul t ures.
Secondly, the exploration of the different orientations between different groups of people toward the IVR di-
mension may enhance the mutual understanding and reduce the disagreement between different people.
To conclude, a clear realization of different tendencies toward IVR dimension of Guangdong Province plays
an important role in successful intercultural communication, which can enhance the mutual understanding, im-
prove working efficiency and he l p c onstruct a ha rmonious soc iety.
2. Literature Review
2.1. Hofstede’s Value Dimension Theory
In 1980, after doing the comprehensive study on how values were influenced by culture in the workplace by in-
vestigating more than 100,000 individuals from 53 countries and regions, Hofstede published his finding, a
theory of cultural value dimensions, which included individualism-collectivism, uncertainty avoidance, power
distance, and masculinity-femininity. The fifth dimension, long-term/short-term orientation, was put forward in
1987.
It’s also worth mentioning that Harry C. Triandis (1988) published his Cross-Cultural Studies of Individual-
ism and Collectivism. In his research, Triandis found that orientations toward individualism and collectivism not
only existed between different cultures but also within one culture, even within each society and each individual.
In 2010, based on Minkov’s analysis of data from the World Value Survey, a new dimension was found,
which was called indulgence-restraint dimension. It becomes the latest dimension of Hofstede’s Value Dimen-
sion Theory. Among the 93 countries that Geert Hofstede and Michael Minkov (2010) calculated the IVR, Chi-
na ranks 75, w hich means that generally China is a restrained culture.
Thus, in this study, the author supposes that the orientations towards IVR also exist within one culture, one
society and one individu a l and trie s to prove it in the empirical stu dy.
2.2. Definition of Indulgence versus Restraint (IVR)
According to Hof stede (2010), the definition of IV R dimens ion is that,
Indulgence stands for a tendency to allow relatively free gratification of basic and natural human desires
related to enjoying life and having fun. Its opposite pole, restraint, ref lects a conviction that such gratifica-
tion needs to be curbed and regulated by strict social norms (Hofstede, 281).
Cai J., Luo J.
118
And it should be noted that the gratification of desires on the indulgence side refers to enjoying life and hav-
ing fun, not t o gr a t i f y i ng human desires in gene ra l .
Thus, one of the two poles of this dimension is characterized by a perception that one can act as one pleases
and indulge in leisurely and fun-related activities with friends or alone. At the other pole we find a perception
that one’s actions are restrained by various social norms and a feeling that leisurely activities, spending, and
other similar types of indulgence are somewhat wrong.
In spite of Hofstede’s IVR dimension, some other related studies have also been carried out. Some of them
share some similarities with IVR, and some of them use IVR to correlate w ith some cultural behaviors and phe-
nomena and explain them.
Michael Bond’s (1992) Chinese Value Survey reported a dimension labeled “moral discipline”. One of its
poles was “moral restraint” and the other is “lack of a strongly disciplined stance”. As quoted by Hofstede,
The items that defined the positive pole of this dimension were ‘moderation’, ‘keeping oneself disinterested
and pure’, and ‘having few desires’. As these items are easy to associate with restraint, one would expect a
negative correlati on wi th indulgenc e (Hofstede , 288).
In 2004, Bond (2004) did another research on what he called social axioms with his associates. They got two
cultural dimensions, one of which was called “societal cynicism”, which is significantly and negatively corre-
lated with IVR. Ulrich Schimmack and Ed Diener (2002) did a survey on how often people experience pleasant
and unpleasant emotions and 6780 college students were involved. The result showed that students in more in-
dulgent societies reported more often experiencing positive emotions. David Schimitt (2005) founded the Inter-
national Sexuality Description Project and one of the cultural studies was sociosexuality. It turned out that
“more indulgent societies have higher percentage of people who have nothing against lax norms concerning ca-
sual sex”.
As for domestic literatures, Fang Ning (2009) carried out an exploratory study on the cultural values of Chi-
na’s post-1980s generation to examine the effects of nationality, employment, gender and age with Hofstede’s
latest VSM08. Sun Qing yan (2013 ) carried out an empirical study to investigate the cultural changes in China in
the aspects of Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Indulgence and Context. Fu Yuxia (2013) did an empir-
ical study on different tendencies of different groups of peop le toward individualism-collectivism dimension by
interview and questionnaires, which provided helpful experience and methodology for this study.
As shown above, most of the related literatures are about the correlation between IVR and different cultural
phenomena. There isn’t any empirical study about IVR. However, this doesn’t mean that there’s no instructive
study for the author. The methodology that Yuxia Fu used in her thesis is useful enough to provide the author
with practical ins tru c tions.
3. Methodology
3.1. Different Defining Attributes of IVR
According to Hofstede and Minkov, some differences between indulgent and restrained societies are summa-
rized in Table 1.
Table 1. Major differences between in dulgent and restrained societies.
Indulgent Society Restrained Society
A perception of personal life control
A perception of helplessness: what happens to me is not my own doing.
Higher importance of leisure Lower importance of leisure
Higher importance of having frie nds
Lower importance of having friends
Thrift is not ve ry important.
Thrift is important.
Loose society Tight society
More likely to remember positive emotions
Less likely to remember positive emotions
Less moral discipline
Moral discipline
Positive attitude Cynicism
More extroverted personalities
More neurotic personalities
Higher optimism
More pessimism
Maintaining order in the nation is not given a high priority Maintaining order in the nation is considered a high priority
Cai J., Luo J.
119
3.2. Research Questions
The present thesis aims to answer the following questions:
1) Is the opinion by Hofstede that China is largely a restraint country, correct in Guangdong Province with the
largest po pulation o f China?
2) Do different orientations toward IVR exist between males and females? And what is the tendency?
3) Do different orientations toward IVR exist between people with different levels of disposable income? And
what is the tendency?
4) Do different orientations toward IVR exist between people from different age groups? And what is the
tendency?
5) Do different orientations toward IVR exist between people from different education background? And
what is the tendenc y?
In order to answer the five questions above, the author has conducted an experimental research with a self-
made questionnaire.
3.3. Subject s
Guangdong is chosen to be the targeted area of this study because of its particularity. Since the reform and
opening policies have been carried out, China has experienced big changes, especially the coastal areas of China,
such as Guangdong. With the rapid economic development, Guangdong has attracted people all around China,
or we can s ay, all around the world to live and work here. It provides a multicultural environment of people liv-
ing in Guangdong. On the other hand, being located next to Hong Kong and Macau makes its location special.
Thus, the author wants to study on the IVR of people in Guangdong, such an open and special area.
Among the 158 questionnaires the author has handed out, there are 150 of them are valid. As the author’s in-
teractive circle is mainly in the west, the east and the south of Guangdong province, most of the questionnaires
were handed out in these areas. In order to ensure the diversity of backgrounds of the subjects, the 150 subjects
involved in the survey are mainly composed of college students, the parents of them and the friends of the par-
ents. What’s more, some people are also asked to finish the questionnaire in some public areas of Guangdong,
such as public parks, supermarkets, etc. The brief information of the subjects is presented in Table 2.
3.4. Design of the Questionnaire
Based on the indulgence-restraint dimension of Hofstede’s Value Dimension Theory, the questionnaire is di-
vided into two parts named part A and part B, which are respectively related to indulgence and restraint. As in-
dulgence-restraint orientation is assumed to exist in every individual, we thus need to compare the scores of
each part and decide whether the individual is more indulgent or more restrained. What’s more, in each part of
the questionnaire, there are three aspects of life related with the statements, which are interpersonal relation,
working style and personal life.
In the experiment, the respondents are asked to choose the most appropriate answer among the five provided
answers which vary from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree” with scores on the scale of 1 to 5. Participants
are familiar with the life-related settings, so they can instinctively decide on the most appropriate answer with
their personal experience.
The study also intends to further check the specific and detailed different IVR orientations. So four variables
are determined, which are genders, age groups, disposable income levels and educational bac kgrounds.
The experiment was carried out mainly by Wechat, the most popular on-line chatting tool in China. As most
of the old don’t use Wechat, the author also did the survey face to face in some public parks and supermarkets,
Table 2. Information of the subjects.
Group
Gender Age Disposable Income
(yuan per month) Education
Male Female 18 - 30 31 - 45 46 - 60 Above
60 Below
2500 2500 - 5000 Above
5000
Below
Vocational
College
Vocational
College
and Above
Number 63 87 72 33 25 20 69 52 29 38 112
Cai J., Luo J.
120
where many old people could be found. At last, 158 questionnaires were completed, 150 of which are valid for
the experiment. All the collected data was analyzed by using SPSS and Excel. The detailed results and data
analysis will be reported in following part.
4. Findings and Discussion
This part mainly focuses on the detailed analyzed data, answering the specific research questions which are
raised in the former part and tries to give reasonable explanations .
4.1. A General Comparison between All the Subjects
In the column 2 of Table 3, we can find that the means scores of part A and B of the 150 subjects are respec-
tively 22.833 an d 20.467. It means that the mean score of part A toward indulgence is slightly higher than that of
part B toward restraint. In column 4, the significance (2-tailed) value is 0.000, much smaller than 0.05. Accord-
ing to the principle of statistics, if P is smaller than 0.05, the diff erence is significant. Thus, the statistic s is mea-
ningful to the study. It indicates that people living in Guangdong generally display a tendency toward indul-
gence, which is different from Hofstede’s point of view that Chinese people are generally restrained. According
to the data, the mean score of each part is higher than the mid-point of the full score, which is 15, indicating that
these subjects present obvious tendencies toward bot h sides of orientations .
Since the reform and opening policies were carried out, China has experienced big changes, especially the
coastal areas of China, such as Guangdong. With the rapid economic development, Guangdong has attracted
people to live and work here. That provides a multicultural environment for people living in Guangdong. On the
other hand, being located next to Hong Kong and Macau is another main factor. Thus, the culture of Guangdong
has been changing from a restraint-oriented one, like the general of China, to a slightly indulgence-oriented one.
According to the third column of Table 4 , the correlation value is −0.11, which means there is a negative re-
lationship between the scores of part A and part B, but the negative relationship is not significant enough, be-
cause −0.11 is slightly less than 0. It indicates that an individual who gets a high mark on indulgence may not
necessarily get low mark on restrain t, and vice versa.
Seeing from the second row of Table 5, we can find that there are 102 subjects whose indulgent scores are
higher than restraint scores, of which 86 subjects’ scores of IVR are both higher than 15. 16 subjects’ indulgent
scores are higher than 15 whereas the restraint scores are lower than 15. The third row of the table shows that
there are 39 subjects whose indulgent scores are lower than the restraint scores. Among them, 31 subjects’
scores of part A & B are both higher than 15, while 8 subjects’ indulgent scores are smaller than or equal to 15
and their restraint scores are higher than 15. Finally we can find from the fourth row that there are only 9 sub-
jects having the same score in both parts and the scores are both higher than 15.
The findings from Table 5 show that: 1) 68% of the subjects display more obvious tendency to indulgence
than to restraint. 16 subjects show a much stronger tendency toward indulgence. 2) Only 39 subjects, 26% of all,
show a more obvious tendency toward restraint than toward indulgence. 3) 126 subjects’ scores of I and R are
both higher than 15, the mid-point of the full score, which means that they have tendencies both to indulgence
and to restraint.
The result has answered the first question mentioned in part three. Hofstede’s opinion that China is largely a
Table 3. Comparison within all subjects.
Part N Mean Score P
A 150 22.833 0.000
B 150 20.467
Notes: A & B is for part A & B of the questionnair e. N is for the number of the subjects.
Table 4. Correlation analysis between part A & B.
Pair N Correlation
A & B 150 0.11
Cai J., Luo J.
121
restraint country now is not correct in Guangdong Province. It also indicates that tendencies toward indulgence
and restraint also exist in an individual for there are individuals who behave both indulgently and restrainedly
and who behave either indulgently or restrainedly.
4.2. Comparative Study between Genders
In this part, the author aims to find out the different tendencies toward IVR from the perspective of genders.
4.2.1. Findings between Genders
As Ta ble 6 shows, the values of P are both 0.000 in the two parts, which are lower than 0.05. It implicates that
there is significant difference between males and females in part A and part B. The mean scores of males and
females in part A are 19.286 and 23.230 respectively, which indicate that males have less tendencies toward in-
dulgence than females. In part B, the mean scores of males and females are respectively 21.508 and 17.713,
which mean that males have more tendencies toward restraint.
4.2.2. Data Analysis between Genders
According to Hofstede (1980), China belongs to masculine society and most men in China have owned higher
social status and more responsibilities in the family and society due to their special social roles since long ago.
Even though Guangdong is the forefront of the reform and opening policy, traditional ideas about males and
Table 5. Different tendencies within all subjects.
Type N Percent
I > R
I > 15 R > 15 86 0.573
I < 15 R < 15 0 0
I > 15 R ≤ 15 16 0.107
Total 102 0.680
I < R
I > 15 R > 15 31 0.207
I < 15 R < 15 0 0
I ≤ 15 R > 15 8 0.053
Total 39 0.260
I = R
I > 15 R > 15 9 0.060
I < 15 R < 15 0 0
I = 15 R = 15 0 0
Total 9 0.060
Total 150 1
Notes: I > R: Indulgence score is higher than restraint score. I < R: Indulgence score is lower than restraint score. I = R:
Indulgence score is equal to restraint score. N: Number of the corresponding subj ects.
Table 6. General difference between genders.
Part Option Mean P
A 1 19.286 0.000
2 23.230
B 1 21.508 0.000
2 17.713
Notes: A : part A of the questionnaire. B: part B of the questionnaire. 1: males. 2: females.
Cai J., Luo J.
122
females haven’t changed a lot. With the development of the economy and the growing cost of living, men are
faced with more and more pressure, which can explain men’s stronger tendencies toward restraint and weaker
tendencies toward indulgence compared with women.
On the other hand, the role of women has actually changed. As women are getting further education, they are
getting a better job and higher status in the society, which offers them more opportunities to choose the life they
want. Thus, according to the statistics, women in Guangdong are changing the traditional stereotypes about
Chinese females. They are even getting more indulgence-oriented compared with men in Guangdong.
4.3. Comparative Study between Different Age Groups
In this chapter, the author intends to find out the different orientations toward IVR dimension from angle of dif-
ferent age groups, including 18 - 30 years old, 31 - 45 years old, 46 - 60 years old and above 60 years old.
4.3.1. Findings between Different Age Groups
As we can see from Table 7, the values of P in part A and B are respectively 0.014 and 0.042, which are both
lower than 0.05 and mean that there are signific ant differences among the subjects. What’s more, we can find in
the table that the mean scores of group 1, 2 and 3 in part A are higher than those in part B, while the mean scores
of group 4 in part A are lower than those in part B. That is to say, that people aged from 18 - 60 hav e more t en-
dencies toward indulgence, while people aged above 60 have more tendencies toward restraint.
4.3.2. Data Analysis between Different Age Grou ps
Figure 1 is formed by the data the author got in Table 7. It shows a specific and vivid comparison between the
Table 7. General difference between different age groups.
Part Option M P
A
1 25.333
0.014
2 23.963
3 22.947
4 17.750
B
1 20.486
0.042
2 21.667
3 22.105
4 25.000
Notes: 1: 18 - 30 y; 2: 31 - 45 y; 3: 46 - 60 y; 4: above 60 y.
Figure 1. Comparisons between the mean scores of different age groups in different parts.
18-30y
31-45y
46-60y
abov e 60y
Part A
Part B
Cai J., Luo J.
123
mean scores of different age groups in different parts. The black pillar refers to the mean score of part A, while
the grey pillar represents the mean score of part B. Thus, we can obviously find from the figure that: 1) The old-
er people are, the less indulgent and the more restrained they become. 2) People aged from 18 to 60 have more
tendencies toward indulgence, while people aged above 60 years old have more tendencies toward restraint.
Subjects in group 1, aged from 18 to 30, are China’s post-1985 generation. They were born after the launch-
ing of the Reform and Opening-up Policy in 1978 and the practice of One Child Policy in 1979. They were
mostly brought up in the one-child family and grew up in the period of rapid economic development. On the one
hand, they grow up in the indulgence from parents and grandparents. On the other hand, they have a broader ho-
rizon of the world. As a result, they tend to be the most indulgent group compared with the other three groups.
Subjects in group 2 and 3 were mostly born after 1970s and 1960s. It’s the generation linking the preceding
and the following and wandering between tradition and modernity. When they were in their childhood, they
were taught that personal interest should be restrained to achieve other greater goals, such as living in a thrift
way to save money for the future. Things began to change when they became teenagers or in their twenties in
the late 1980s. With a better economic environment and foreign ideas rushing into their mind due to the reform
and opening-up policy, they realized that the pursuit of happiness and enjoying life are one’s individual rights
and indulgence is acceptable. Therefore, they also have more tendencies toward indulgence, but the tendencies
are weaker compared with those of group 1.
In group 4, subjects were mainly born after 1950s in big families. Life was tougher in most of their life and
they are more close-minded. Thus, this group tend s to restrain themselves. Even now their life is getting better;
they still tend to be restrained instead of being indulgent, just as it’s become something deeply rooted in their
mind.
4.4. Comparative Study between People with Different Disposable Incomes and
Different Educational Backgrounds
4.4.1. Findings between People with Different Disposable Incomes
Table 8 shows that in part A and B, there are no significant differences between different disposable incomes
levels, because the values of P are respectively 0.061 and 0.524, which are both higher than 0.05. What’s more,
the mean scores of this restraint part are very close to each other. However, the data indicates that all the sub-
jects in different disposable incomes levels show a tendency toward indulgence because the mean scores in part
A are all higher than those in part B. That is to say that, no matter how much disposable income the subjects get
every month, they tend to be indulgent.
4.4.2. Findings between People with Different Educational Backgrounds
Table 9 shows that the value of P in the indulgence-oriented part is 0.065, higher than 0.05, and so is it in the
restraint-oriented part (0.089). It means that both educational backgrounds show no significant difference in in-
dulgence and restraint. It can be concluded by comparing the mean scores that the general subjects have tenden-
cies toward indulgence.
Table 8. General difference between people with different disposable incomes.
Part Option M P
A
1 23.130
0.061
2 21.865
3 23.862
B
1 20.507
0.524
2 20.538
3 20.241
Notes: 1: below 2500 y ua n per m ont h; 2: 2 50 0 - 5000 yuan per month; 3: above 5000 yuan per month .
Cai J., Luo J.
124
Table 9. General difference between people with different educational backgrounds.
Part Option M P
A 1 22.368 0.065
2 23.009
B 1 21.868 0.089
2 18.991
Notes: 1 : less th a n voc at ional college; 2: v oca ti o nal co ll ege a nd a bove .
4.4.3. Data Analysis between People with Different Disposable Incomes and Different Educational
Backgrounds
According to the data shown in Table 8 and Table 9, subjects with different disposable income and different
educational backgrounds have no significant differences in general. And they generally have the tendency to-
wards indulgence.
With the rapid development of Guangdong, people’s living standard has increased. Thus, the disposable in-
come doesn’t significantly affect people’s behavior in IVR. For example, even though people with lower dis-
posable income cannot afford luxury goods, which may be regarded as one of the ways of indulgence, they still
live a comparatively indulgent life in their way, such as being together with whom they love or buying some-
thing they want, which doesn’t have to be very expensive. It’s also the same reason for people with different
educational backgr ou nds.
All in all, in a rapidly developing and opened area like Guangdong, no matter how people are comparatively
rich or poor, comparatively high or low educated, they generally behave in a comparatively indulgence-oriented
way.
5. Conclusions
Based on Hofstede’s value dimension theory, the following findings are produced by analyzing the data in the
present study.
First of all, the subjects generally show a slightly stronger tendency toward indulgence than to restraint. Thus,
the idea that China is largely a restraint country is incorrect in Guangdong Province. With respect to genders,
males have fewer tendencies toward indulgence and more tendencies toward restraint than females. Considering
age groups, older people have more tendencies toward restraint and fewer tendencies toward indulgence. What’s
more, people aged from 18 to 60 are generally indulgence-oriented while people aged above 60 years old are
generally restraint-oriented. It is also found that people with different disposable incomes and educational back-
grounds have no significant difference in general. All of them, generally, tend to be indulgent.
References
Bond, M. (1992). The Process of Enhancing Cross-Cultural Competence in Hong Kong Organizations. International Journal
of Intercultural Relations, 16, 395-412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(92)90030-X
Bond, M. (2004). Culture-Level Dimensions of Social Axioms and Their Correlates across 41 Cultures. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 35, 548-570. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022104268388
Fu Yuxia (2013). An Empirical Study on Different Tendencies toward Individualism and CollectivismFrom Inter-Cultural
Perspective. Thesis, Hefei: Anhui University.
Hofstede, G., & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind. London: McGraw-Hill.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Difference in Work-Related Value. Hills, CA: Sage Publica-
tions.
Ning Fang (2009). An Exploratory Study on the Cultural Values of Chinas P ost-1980s GenerationExamining the Effects
of Nationality, Employment, Gender and Age with Hofstede’s Latest VSM08. Thesis, Shanghai: Shanghai International
Studies University.
Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural Influences on the Relation between Pleasant Emotions and Unpleasant Emo-
tions: Asian Dialectic Philosophies or Individualism-Collectivism? Cognition and Emotion, 16, 705-719.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000590
Cai J., Luo J.
125
Schimitt, D. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-Nation Study of Sex, Culture, and Strategies of Hu-
man Mating. Behavioral and Brain Science, 28, 247-311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x05000051
Sun Qingyan (2013). The Changes of Cultural Patterns in China. Thesis, Xi’an: Xi’an International Studies University.
Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism vs. Individualism: A Reconceptualization of a Basic Concept in Cross-Cultural Psy-
chology. Cross-Cultural Studies of Personality, Attitudes and Cognition, 60-95.
Cai J., Luo J.
126
Appendix
Questionnaire about Guangdong People’s Different Tendencies toward IVR
Thank you for your cooperation! As this questionnaire is anonymous, your answer is only for academic research
without for any other purpose. According to your actual situation, please choose the most suitable answer and
provide your real gender, age, disposable income and educational background. Scores range from 5 to 1 accord-
ing to different options. Thank you!
Basic Information:
1. Are you G u a ngdong inha b itant?
Yes ____ No ____
2. Gend er:
Female ____ Male ____
3. Age gro up:
18 - 30 years old ____ 31 - 45 years old ____
46 - 60 years old ____ above 60 years old____
4. Monthly disposable income level (If student, please choose your monthly consumption level):
less than 2500 Yu an __ __ 2500 - 5000 Yuan ____ more than 5000 Yuan__ _ _
5. Educational background:
below vocational college ____ vocational college and above ____
Part A
1. I can decide on my life partner and I think that choosing the one I love is the most important thing.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
2. I like making friends with funny and free people. They bring happiness and surprise to life, even though
sometimes they are in lack of plans.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strong ly disagree___
3. I stop to have a rest when I feel tired at work.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
4. I like relaxing working environment and atmosphere, where colleagues can talk freely and frequently.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
5. I work hard and earn money in order to have higher-quality life, so I would make some spontaneous deci-
sions, such as a quick-decided trip, even though I’m not very rich at that time.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
6. I like living for now and I think happiness is the most important thing, so we don’t have to do anything that
we don’t like because of others’ comments.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
Part B
1. Other people’s opinions strongly affect my decision on my life partner.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
2. I like being with strict and self-disciplined people. They keep life in order and make everything go as
planned.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
3. I would forget about food and rest be c au se of work.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
4. I think serious working environment is better for the working efficiency.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
5. I live a simple life style and save money for emergency.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___
6. When I’m out with friends, I always keep my unfinished homework or work in mind.
Strongly agree___ Agree___ Not sure___ Disagree___ Strongly disa gree ___