Emotion Ownership: Different Effects on Explicit Ratings and Implicit Responses

Abstract

This study is based on the idea that emotion-related processing happens on various different levels. Common methods that are used to measure different aspects of emotion-related processing exhibit specific sensitivities to one or the other of these separate processing levels. Here, the hypothesis was tested that explicit and implicit measures of emotion-related processing are differently influenced by self-referenced versus unreferenced emotion. The explicit measure was self reported valence and the implicit measure was startle reflex modulation. In one session, emotional scenes were paired with short sentences inducing self-reference (e.g. “this dog will attack you” written underneath the image of an aggressive dog), while in another session, emotional scenes were presented without any reference. During both sessions explicit as well as implicit responses were collected. Self-referenced unpleasant images were rated more negative and self-referenced pleasant images were rated more positive than images with no reference. In contrast, the implicit measure indicated greater startle responses related to self-reference regardless of emotion category. Under the common assumption that greater startle responses reflect increased affective negativity, this means that self-referenced pleasant images elicited more negative implicit affect than unreferenced pleasant images. However, in both cases (self-referenced and unreferenced) startle responses demonstrated valence depended modulation as expected. Thus, in our study startle responses demonstrated sensitivity to affective valence as well as self-reference. It is concluded that self-reference is linked to increased motivation, which in turn has been reported to be detectable via startle reflex modulation (SRM) as well.

Share and Cite:

Walla, P. , Rosser, L. , Scharfenberger, J. , Duregger, C. & Bosshard, S. (2013). Emotion Ownership: Different Effects on Explicit Ratings and Implicit Responses. Psychology, 4, 213-216. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.43A032.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Arthur-Kelly, M., Lyons, G. S., & Walla, P. (2012). Toward improved ways of knowing children with multiple disabilities (PMD): Introducing startle reflex modulation. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, in press.
[2] Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioural Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49-59. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9
[3] Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M., McManis, M., & Lang, P. J. (1998). Probing affective pictures: Attended startle and tone probes. Psychophysiology, 35, 344-347. doi:10.1017/S0048577298970536
[4] Dunning, J. P., Auriemmo, A., Castille, C., & Hajcak, G. (2010). In the face of anger: Startle modulation to graded facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 47, 874-878.
[5] Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Mehta, N., Kring, A. M., & Patrick. C. J. (2007). Impact of motivational salience on affect modulated startle at early and late probe times. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 266-270. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.005
[6] Geiser, M., & Walla, P. (2011). Objective measures of emotion during virtual walks through urban environments. Applied Sciences, 1, 1-11.
[7] Grahl, A., Greiner, U., & Walla, P. (2012). Bottle shape elicits gender-specific emotion: A startle reflex modulation study. Psychology, 3, 548-554. doi:10.4236/psych.2012.37081
[8] Grillon, C., & Baas, J. (2003) A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application in psychiatry. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 1557-1579. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00202-5
[9] Herbert, C., Herbert, B. M., Ethofer, T., & Pauli, P. (2011a) His or mine? The time course of self-other discrimination in emotion processing. Social Neuroscience, 6, 277-288. doi:10.1080/17470919.2010.523543
[10] Herbert, C., Herbert, B. M., & Pauli, P. (2011b). Emotional self-reference: Brain structures involved in the processing of words describing one’s own emotions. Neurophsycologia, 49, 2947-2956. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.026
[11] Jackson, D. C., Malmstadt, J. R., Larson, C. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Suppression and enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 37, 515-522. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3740515
[12] Junghofer, M., Kissler, J., Schupp, H. T., Putsche, C., Elling, L., & Dobel, C. (2010). A fast neural signature of motivated attention to consumer goods separates the sexes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 179. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00179
[13] Koller, M., & Walla, P. (2012). Measuring affective information processing in information systems and consumer research—Introducing startle reflex modulation. ICIS 2012 Proceedings, in press.
[14] Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8, Gainesville, FL: University of Florida,.
[15] Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248-1263. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00275-3
[16] Mühlberger, A., Wieser, M. J., & Pauli, P. (2008). Darkness-enhanced startle responses in ecologically valid environments: A virtual tunnel driving experiment. Biological Psychology, 77, 47-52. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.004
[17] Vrana S R, Spence E L & Lang P J (1988) The startle probe response: a new measure of emotion? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 487-491. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.4.487
[18] Walla, P., & Panksepp, J. (2013). Neuroimaging helps to clarify affective processing without necessarily clarifying emotions. In K. N. Fountas, (Ed.), Novel frontiers of advanced neuroimaging. Intech. http://www.intechopen.com/books/novel-frontiers-of-advanced-neuroiming/neuroimaging-helps-to-clarify-brain-affective-processingwith out-necessarily-clarifying-emotions
[19] Walla, P., Brenner, G., & Koller, M. (2011). Objective measures of emotion related to brand attitude: A new way to quantify emotion-related aspects relevant to marketing. Plos One, 6, e26782. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026782
[20] Walla, P., Richter, M., Farber, S., Leodolter, U., & Bauer, H. (2010). Food evoked changes in humans: Startle response modulation and event-related potentials (ERPs). Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 25-32. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000003
[21] Walla, P., Duregger, C., Greiner, K., Thurner, S., & Ehrenberger, K. (2008). Multiple aspects related to self awareness and the awareness of others: An electroencephalography (EEG) study. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115, 983-992. doi:10.1007/s00702-008-0035-6
[22] Walla, P., Greiner, K., Duregger, C., Deecke, L., & Thurner, S. (2007). Self-awareness and the subconscious effect of personal pronouns on word encoding: A magnetoencephalography (MEG) study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 796-809. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.017

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.