Physical Activity Improves Mental Rotation Performance
Petra Jansen, Stefanie Pietsch
.
DOI: 10.4236/ce.2010.11009   PDF    HTML     6,431 Downloads   11,964 Views   Citations

Abstract

Even there seemed to be general knowledge that physical activity enhanced spatial cognitive performance almost none experimental studies on this influence exist. For that the influence of physical activity on mental rotation per-formance is investigated in this study. Mental rotation is the ability to imagine how an object would look if rotated away from the original orientation. Two groups of 44 students of educational science each solved a psychometrical mental rotation task with three-dimensional block figures. After this, the participants of the physical activity group took part in a sport lesson, whereas the participants of the cognitive activity group attended an oral lesson of kinematics. Both lessons took 45 minutes. Thereafter, all participants solved the mental rotation task again. The results showed that the participants of the physical activity group improved their mental rotation performance whereas the participants of the cognitive activity showed no improvement.

Share and Cite:

Jansen, P. & Pietsch, S. (2010). Physical Activity Improves Mental Rotation Performance. Creative Education, 1, 58-61. doi: 10.4236/ce.2010.11009.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] J. Piaget, “The Origins of Intelligence in the Child (1936, 1963),” (La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant, M. Cook, Trans.), International Universities Press, W.W. Norton & Company, New York, 1956.
[2] A. Sheya and L. B. Smith, “Development through Sen-sory-Motor Coordinations,” Manuscript Submitted for Publication, 2008.
[3] L. B. Smith, E. Thelen, R. Titzer and D. McLin, “Knowing in the Context of Acting: the Task Dynamics of the A-Not-B Error,” Psychological Review, Vol. 106, No. 2, 1999, pp. 235-260.
[4] C. Fabre, K. Chamari, P. Mucci, J. Massé-Biron and C. Préfaut, “Improvement of Cognitive Function by Mental and/or Individualized Aerobic Training in Healthy Elderly Subjects,” International Journal of Sports Medicine, Vol. 23, No. 6, 2002, pp. 415-421.
[5] C. Gillberg, “Deficits in Attention, Motor Control, and Perception: A Brief Review [Oct],” Archives of disease in childhood, Vol. 88, No. 10, 2003, pp. 904-910.
[6] M. R. Leary, “Moving on: Autism and Movement Dis-turbance,” Mental Retardation, Vol. 34, No. 1, 1996, pp. 39-53.
[7] T. M. Pitcher, J. P. Piek and D. A. Hay, “Fine and Gross Motor Ability in Males with ADHD,” Developmental medicine and child neurology, Vol. 45, No. 8, 2003, pp. 525-535.
[8] J. L. Etnier, P. M. Nowell, D. M. Landers and B. A. Sib-ley, “A Meta-Regression to Examine the Relationship between Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Performance,” Brain Research Reviews, Vol. 52, No. 1, 2006, pp. 119- 130.
[9] J. Etnier, W. Salazar, D. Landers and S. Petruzello, “The Influence of Physical Fitness and Exercise upon Cognitive Functioning: A Meta-Analysis,” Journal of Exercise & Sport Psychology, Vol. 19, No. 3, 1997, pp. 249-277.
[10] A. Diamond, “Interrelated and Interdependent,” Deve-lopmental Science, Vol. 10, No. 1, 2007, pp. 152-158.
[11] J. J. Campos, D. I. Anderson, M. A. B. Roth, E. M. Hub-bard, M. J. Hertenstein and D. Witherington, “Travel Broadens the Mind,” Infancy, Vol. 1, No. 2, 2000, pp. 149-219.
[12] M. C. Linn and A. C. Peterson, “Emergence and Charac-terization of Sex-Differences in Spatial Ability: A Meta- Analysis,” Child Development, Vol. 56, No. 6, 1985, pp. 1479-1498.
[13] R. N. Shepard and J. Metzler, “Mental Rotation of Three- Dimensional Objects,” Science, Vol. 171, No. 972, 1971, pp. 701-703.
[14] D. C. Geary, S. J. Saults, F. Liu and M. K. Hoard, “Sex Differences in Spatial Cognition, Computational Fluency, and Arithmetical Reasoning,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Vol. 77, No. 4, 2000, pp. 337-353.
[15] M. Hegarty and M. Kozhevnikov, “Types of Visual-Spa-tial Representations and Mathematical Problem Solving,” Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 91, No. 4, 1999, pp. 684-689.
[16] M. Peters, P. Chisholm and B. Laeng, “Spatial Ability, Student Gender and Academic Performance,” Journal of Engineering Education, Vol. 84, No. 1, 1995, pp. 69-73.
[17] P. Jansen, C. Titze and M. Heil, “The Influence of Jug-gling on Mental Rotation Performance,” Journal of In-ternational Sport Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 1, 2009, pp. 351-359.
[18] M. Wraga, W. L. Thompson, N. M. Alpert and S. M. Kosslyn, “Implicit Transfer of Motor Strategies in Mental Rotation,” Brain and Cognition, Vol. 52, No. 2, 2003, pp. 135-143.
[19] B. Draganski, C. Gaser, V. Busch, G. Schuierer, U. Bog-dahn and A. May, “Neuroplasticity: Changes in Grey Matter Induced by Training,” Nature, Vol. 427, No. 6972, 2004, pp. 311-312.
[20] K. Jordan, J. Heinze, K. Lutz, M. Kanowski and L. J?ncke, “Cortical Activations during the Mental Rotation of Different Visual Objects,” Neuroimage, Vol. 13, No. 1, 2001, pp. 143-152.
[21] S. G. Vandenberg and A. R. Kuse, “Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 47, No. 2, 1978, pp. 599-604.
[22] M. Casey, R. L. Nuttall and E. Pezaris, “Mediators of Gender Differences in Mathematics College Entrance Test Scores: A Comparison of Spatial Skills with Internalized Beliefs and Anxieties,” Developmental Psychology, Vol. 33, No. 4, 1997, pp. 669-680.
[23] L. Nilges, “The Role of Spatial Ability in Physical Edu-cation and Mathematics,” The Journal of Physical Edu-cation, Recreation & Dance, Vol. 71, No. 6, 2000, pp. 29-32.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.