Processing Facilitation Strategies in OV and VO Languages: A Corpus Study

Abstract

The present corpus study aimed to examine whether Basque (OV) resorts more often than Spanish (VO) to certain grammatical operations, in order to minimize the number of arguments to be processed before the verb. Ueno & Polinsky (2009) argue that VO/OV languages use certain grammatical resources with different frequencies in order to facilitate real-time processing. They observe that both OV and VO languages in their sample (Japanese, Turkish and Spanish) have a similar frequency of use of subject pro-drop; however, they find that OV languages (Japanese, Turkish) use more intransitive sentences than VO languages (English, Spanish), and conclude this is an OV-specific strategy to facilitate processing. We conducted a comparative corpus study of Spanish (VO) and Basque (OV). Results show (a) that the frequency of use of subject pro-drop is higher in Basque than in Spanish; and (b) Basque does not use more intransitive sentences than Spanish; both languages have a similar frequency of intransitive sentences. Based on these findings, we conclude that the frequency of use of grammatical resources to facilitate the processing does not depend on a single typological trait (VO/OV) but it is modulated by the concurrence of other grammatical features.

Share and Cite:

Pastor, L. and Laka, I. (2013) Processing Facilitation Strategies in OV and VO Languages: A Corpus Study. Open Journal of Modern Linguistics, 3, 252-258. doi: 10.4236/ojml.2013.33033.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279 362), New York, NY: Wiley and Sons.
[2] Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[3] De Rijk, & Rudolf, P.G. (2007). Standard Basque: A progressive gram mar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[4] Dryer, M. S. (2011). Order of subject, object, and verb. In M. Haspel math, M. S. Dryer, D. Gil, & B. Comrie (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. http://wals.info/chapter/81
[5] Erdozia, K., Laka, I., Mestres-Misse, A., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Syntactic complexity and ambiguity resolution in a free word order language: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidences from Basque. Brain and Language, 109, 1-17. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.12.003
[6] Garnseya, S. M., Pearlmutterb, N. J., Myersa, Elizabeth, & Lotockyc, M. A. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Me mory and Language, 37, 58-93. doi:10.1006/jmla.1997.2512
[7] Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of Language (pp. 40-70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[8] Hawkins, J. A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[9] Hawkins, J. A. (2003). Efficiency and complexity in grammars: Three general principles. In J. Moore, & M. Polinsky (Eds.), The Nature of Explanation in Linguistic Theory (pp. 121-152). Stanford: CSLI Pub lications, Stanford University.
[10] Hualde, J. I., & Ortiz de Urbina, J. (2003). A Grammar of Basque. Ber lin: Mouton de Gruyter.
doi:10.1515/9783110895285
[11] Kameyama, M. (1985). Zero anaphora: The case of Japanese. Doctoral Dissertation, Stanford, CA: Stanford University.
[12] Kameyama, M. (1988). Japanese zero pronominal binding: Where syn tax and discourse meet. In W. J. Poser (Ed.), 2nd International Work shop on Japanese Syntax (pp. 47-73). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publica tions.
[13] Laka, I. (1996). A brief grammar of Euskara, the Basque language.
www.ei.ehu.es/p289-content/eu/contenidos/informacion/grammar_euskara/en_doc/index.html
[14] Lindsley, J. R. (1975). Producing simple utterances: How far ahead do we plan? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 1-19. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90002-X
[15] MacDonald, M. C. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1-16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226
[16] Pickering, M. J. (1993). Direct association and sentence processing: A reply to Gorrell and to Gibson and Hickok. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 163-196. doi:10.1080/01690969308406953
[17] Pickering, M. J., & Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without emp ty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 229-259. doi:10.1080/01690969108406944
[18] Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical pref erence from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528-553. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.3.528
[19] Prince, E. F. (1999). Subject pro-drop in Yiddish. In P. Bosch, & R. van der Sandt (Eds.), Focus: Linguistic, cognitive, and computatio nal perspectives (pp. 82-104). Cambridge, CA: Cambridge Universi ty Press.
[20] Pritchett, B. L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing perfor mance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
[21] Ros, I., Laka, I., Fukumura, K., & Santesteban, M. (2012). Long-be fore-short in head-final languages that agree. The Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP), Uni versidad di Trento.
[22] Turan, ü. D. (1998). Ranking forward-looking centers in Turkish: Uni versal and language specific properties. In M. A. Walker, A. K. Joshi, & E. F. Prince (Eds.), Centering theory in discourse (pp. 139-161). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
[23] Ueno, M., & Polinsky, M. (2009). Does headedness affect processing? A new look at the VO-OV contrast. Journal of Linguistics, 45, 675 710. doi:10.1017/S0022226709990065
[24] Walker, M. A., Iida, M., & Cote, S. (1994). Japanese discourse and the process of centering. Computacional Linguistics, 20, 193-231.
[25] Yamashita, H., & Chang, F. (2001). “Long before short” preference in the production of a head-final language. Cognition, 81, B45-B55. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00121-4
[26] Yamashita, H., Chang, F., & Hirose, Y. (2005). Producers build struc tures only with overt arguments. The 18th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Tucson.
[27] Yngve, V. H. (1960). A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 140, 444-466.

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.