Rumination, Mood and Cognitive Performance

Abstract

Rumination, DEFINED AS REPETITIVE, RECURRENT AND UNCONTROLLABLE THINKING, has been implicated in cognitive impairments, particularly dysexecutive function, in people with depressed mood, what is now critical is an examination of the link between rumination and cognitive impairment independent of mood. Rumination’s direct relationship to basic cognitive functions may help explain the stability of rumination, and how it predicts future episodes of depressed mood. Two experiments examined how trait and experimentally induced rumination, and negative mood relate to attention and inhibition at varying levels of cognitive load in non-depressed participants. Study 1 found trait rumination significantly predicted errors of attention when cognitive load was low or high, but not at a medium level. Trait rumination also interacted with frequency of task unrelated thoughts to predict performance. Negative mood significantly predicted errors of inhibition also when the cognitive load was low or high. Study 2 expanded on the first by including two additional measures of executive function. Results were replicated for the first study and showed a ruminative thought style predicted inhibition, reaction time and set shifting in the additional measures.

Share and Cite:

Brinker, J. , Campisi, M. , Gibbs, L. & Izzard, R. (2013). Rumination, Mood and Cognitive Performance. Psychology, 4, 224-231. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.43A034.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus-independent thought. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 423-430. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1968.tb01157.x
[2] Austin, M., Mitchell, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 200-206. doi:10.1192/bjp.178.3.200
[3] Brinker, J. K., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2009). Ruminative thought style and depressed mood. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 1-19. doi:10.1002/jclp.20542
[4] Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499-506. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499
[5] Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and non-ruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 699-711. doi:10.1023/A:1005591412406
[6] Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigel-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404-411. doi:10.1037/h0059831
[7] Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2003). To err is autonomic: Error-related brain potentials, ANS activity, and post-error compensatory behavior. Psychophysiology, 40, 895-903. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00107
[8] Hertel, P. T. (1997). On the contributions of deficient cognitive control to memory impairments in depression. Cognition & Emotion, 11, 569-584. doi:10.1080/026999397379890a
[9] Joorman, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventative Psychology, 12, 128-139. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.002
[10] Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2001). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 138-145. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005
[11] Lau, M., Christensen, B. K., Hawley, L. L., Gemar, M. S., & Segal, Z. V. (2007). Inhibitory deficits for negative information in persons with major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1249-1259. doi:10.1017/S0033291707000530
[12] Lo, B. C. Y., Lau, S., Cheung, S., & Allen, N. B. (2012). The impact of rumination on internal attention switching. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 209-203. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.574997
[13] Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 309-330. doi:10.1023/A:1023918517378
[14] Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339-349. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.339
[15] Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176-190. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176
[16] Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1-47). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[17] Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex “frontal-lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
[18] Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638-662. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
[19] Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Parker, L., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92-104. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.92
[20] Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400-424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
[21] Papageorgiou, C., & Siegle, G. J. (2003). Rumination and depression: Advances in theory and research. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 243-245. doi:10.1023/A:1023918331490
[22] Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673-1681. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002
[23] Rogers, M. A., Kasai, K., Matsuo, K., Fukuda, R., Iwanami, A., Naka-gome, K., Fukuda, M., & Kato, N. (2004). Executive and prefrontal dysfuction in unipolar depression: A review of neuropsychological and imaging evidence. Neuroscience Research, 50, 1-11. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2004.05.003
[24] Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition, 23, 551-559. doi:10.3758/BF03197257
[25] Verhaeghen, P., Joorman, J., & Khan, R. (2005). Why we sing the blues: The relation between self-reflective rumination, mood, and creativity. Emotion, 5, 226-232. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.226
[26] Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72, 400-402. doi:10.1136/jnnp.72.3.400
[27] Watkins, E., & Moulds, M., (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319-328. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319
[28] Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa. http://www.psychology.uiowa.edu/faculty/clark/panas-x.pdf
[29] Watson, P. J., & Andrews, P. W. (2002). Toward a revised evolutionary adaptionist analysis of depression: The social navigation hypothesis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 1-14. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00459-1
[30] Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18, 546-553. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01936.x
[31] Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Rau, H. K. (2009) Individual differences in executive functioning: Implications for stress regulation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 126-140. doi:10.1007/s12160-009-9100-0

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.